I
*
United States
Environmental Protection
Agency
Office of Health and
Environmental Assessment
Washington DC 20460
                                            EPA/600/8-89/049A
                                            March 1989
                                            CASAC Review Draft
Research and Development
Supplement to the
1986 EPA Air Quality
Criteria for Lead -
Volume I Addendum
(pages A1-A67)
                Review
                Draft

                (Do Not
                Cite or Quote)
                 NOTICE
This document is a preliminary draft. It has not been formally released by EPA and
should not at this stage be construed to represent Agency policy. It is being
circulated for comment on its technical accuracy and policy implications.

-------
DRAFT                                    EPA/600/8-89/049A
DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE                             March 1989
                                         CAS AC Review Draft
        Supplement to the 1986
       EPA Air Quality Criteria for
      Lead -Volume I Addendum
               (pages A1-A67)
                        NOTICE
  This document is a preliminary draft. It has not been formally released by the U.S.
  Environmental Protection agency and Should not at this stage be construed to
  represent Agency policy. It is being circulated for comment on its technical accuracy
  and policy implications.
         Office of Health and Environmental Assessment
             Office of Research and Development
             U.S. Environmental Protection Agency
                  Washington, DC 20460
                                 Eiwfowmental Protection ***»
                                       anf(Pt-l2£.
                                      son Bou^aw
                                      60604-3590

-------
                                  DISCLAIMER







     This document is an  external  draft for review purposes only and does not



constitute Agency policy.  Mention  of trade names or commercial  products does



not constitute endorsement or recommendation for use.
                                      ii

-------
                                   CONTENTS
TABLES 	        iv
FIGURES 	        vi
ABSTRACT	      vi i
AUTHORS 	     vi i i

  I.  INTRODUCTION	         1

 II.  RELATIONSHIP OF BLOOD PRESSURE TO LEAD EXPOSURE	         2

III.  NEUROBEHAVIORAL AND GROWTH EFFECTS IN INFANTS AND CHILDREN  ..        23

 IV.  REFERENCES 	        63

-------
                                    TABLES

Number                                                                    Page

  1      Coefficients for the natural  log of blood lead
         concentration (logPbB) vs.  blood pressure (BP) in
         men with and without adjustment for site variables 	         8

 2       Infants' mental development index scores according
         to cord blood lead group 	        24

 3       Change in McCarthy general  cognitive index (GCI) and
         subscale scores associated  with each natural  log unit
         increase in blood lead level  	        26

 4       Results of multiple regression analyses examining
         the relationship between blood lead level and
         performance on the Bayley Mental Index at 3-24 months
         of age 	        28

 5A      The relationship of fetal (maternal and cord) PbB with
         later developmental outcomes  	        33

 5B      The relationship of prior and current postnatal PbB
         with later developmental outcomes 	        33

 6       The relationship of prenatal  and postnatal log blood
         lead with IQ at age four years, ten months 	        34

 7       Estimated coefficients of log blood lead concentration
         from simple and multiple regression analyses  of McCarthy
         scores at the age of four years 	        37

 8       Regression of developmental indices on maternal and
         cord blood 1 ead 1 eve! s 	        39

 9       Regression of developmental indices at 48 months on
         current and prior blood levels 	        41

10A      Significant bivariate correlations of 30-day NBAS
         trend and lead measures 	        43

10B      Effect of addition of lead  to the stepwise multiple
         regression model using prior entry of all control
         variables that have significant bivariate correlations
         with 30-day trend of NBAS 	        43

11       Unexplained correlations between dentine lead levels
         (log ug/g) taking into account test reliability,
         confounding covariates, sample selection factors, and
         reverse causality via pica  	        46
                                      IV

-------
                                    TABLES

Number                                                                    Page

12       Product moment correlations between maternal,  teacher
         behaviour ratings and dentine lead values (log pg/g) 	        47

13       Strongest relationships between blood lead measures
         at specified times and later neurobehavioral  outcomes
         as detected by prospective studies 	        60

-------
                                    FIGURES

Number                                                                   Page

  1      Comparison of study results from four larger-scale
         epidemiology studies of lead-blood pressure relation-
         ships in adult men.   BRHS = British Regional Heart
         Study analyses, described by Pocock et al.  (1988);
         NHANES II = National Health and Nutrition Evaluation
         Survey analyses described by Schwartz (1988);
         Caerphilly and Wales = Welsh studies described by
         Elwood et al.  (1988a,b)	     14
                                      VI

-------
                                 ABSTRACT
     The 1986 U.S.  EPA document Air Quality Criteria for Lead
(EPA-600/8-83/028 aF-dF) evaluated in detail the latest scientific
information concerning sources, routes, and levels of lead (Pb) exposure
and associated health effects and potential risks.   An Addendum (1986)
to that document focuses on additional, newer studies concerning the
effects of lead on cardiovascular function and on early physical and
neurobehavioral development.   The present Supplement to the above
materials evaluates further still newer information emerging in the
published literature concerning (1) lead effects on blood pressure and
other cardiovascular endpoints and (2) the effects of lead exposure
during pregnancy or early postnatally on birth outcomes and/or the
neonatal physical and neuropsychological development of affected
children.  The evaluations contained in this Supplement and the 1986
Criteria Document and Addendum are to serve as scientific inputs to
decisionmaking with regard to review and revision,  as appropriate, of
the National  Ambient Air Quality Standards (NAAQS) for Lead.

-------
                                  AUTHORS
Dr.  J.  Michael Davis, Environmental  Criteria and Assessment Office
     (MD-52), OHEA, ORD, U.S.  Environmental  Protection Agency,
     Research Triangle Park,  NC 27711

Dr.  Lester D. Grant, Environmental  Criteria  and Assessment Office
     (MD-52), OHEA, ORD, U.S.  Environmental  Protection Agency,
     Research Triangle Park,  NC 27711

Dr.  Winona W. Victery, Environmental Criteria and Assessment Office
     (MD-52), OHEA, ORD, U.S.  Environmental  Protection Agency,
     Research Triangle Park,  NC 27711
                                 REVIEWERS
Dr. David Bellinger, Harvard Medical  School, Children's Hospital,
     Boston, MA 02115

Dr. Kim N.  Dietrich, Institute of Environmental Health, University of
     Cincinnati, College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH 45267-0056

Dr. David J. Svendsgaard, Health Effects Research Laboratory (MD-55),
     OHR, ORD, U.S.  Environmental Protection Agency, Research Triangle
     Park,  NC 27711
                                     vm

-------
          SUPPLEMENT TO THE 1986 EPA LEAD CRITERIA DOCUMENT ADDENDUM
I.  INTRODUCTION
     In the mid-1980's,  the  1977 EPA criteria document, Air Quality  Criteria
for Lead  (U.S.  EPA,  1977) was  updated and  revised pursuant  to  Sections  108-109
of the Clean  Air  Act,  as amended, 42 U.S.C.  7408 and 7409.   The final version
of the updated criteria document (U.S.  EPA, 1986a) incorporating revisions made
in response to  public  comments and CASAC  review  of  earlier drafts, was com-
pleted in  1986  to be used as a basis for review and, as appropriate,  revision
of the National Ambient  Air  Quality Standard (NAAQS)  for  lead.   An Addendum
(U.S.  EPA,  1986b)  to the revised document, Air Quality. Criteria  for Lead  (U.S.
EPA, 1986a),  was  also  completed  in 1986  and  evaluated  newly published informa-
tion concerning two topics:   (1) the relationship between blood lead levels and
cardiovascular effects;  and  (2)  lead exposure effects  on early development and
stature.
     In the  three years  since the 1986 Addendum was  prepared,  information
pertaining to the health effects of lead has  continued to emerge with regard to
the topics  addressed in  that  Addendum.   Although newer findings  have been
generally consistent with the  state of understanding  that  was articulated in
the 1986  Addendum, they  still  need to be evaluated as part  of an updated
assessment of the  latest scientific information that characterizes the health
effects of  lead  (Pb)  with  clear  relevance  for  decisionmaking on  potential
revision of the existing Lead NAAQS.
     The present  update  focuses  primarily  on key  findings that have emerged in
the lead  literature since 1986  in the  areas of lead  effects  on:  (1) blood
pressure and  related cardiovascular endpoints and (2)  child development.   It
does not  attempt  to  be comprehensive in  reviewing this literature,  nor does it
attempt to  address all  the  topics  covered in the 1986 Air Quality Criteria
Document and  Addendum.   Rather,  its explicit purpose is to  describe and inter-
pret the critical  effects of lead thft.  have greatest significance for impending
regulatory decisions regarding this environmental pollutant.
3/27/89

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


II.   RELATIONSHIP OF BLOOD PRESSURE TO LEAD EXPOSURE
     The 1986 Addendum (U.S.  EPA,  1986b)  addressed the issue  of lead  effects  on
the cardiovascular system  in  a review of the findings with  regard to overtly
lead-intoxicated  individuals,  epidemiologic  studies  of associations  between
lead exposure and increased blood  pressure,  toxicologic data  providing evidence
for  lead-induced  cardiovascular  effects   in  animals,  and  information on the
possible mechanisms of action  of  lead on cardiovascular function.  The amount
and  sufficiency  of  the  literature at that time were  adequate to provide an
initial  evaluation  of the topic,  but did  not  come to a fully definitive
consensus with  regard to  the  contributory  role  of lead exposure to  hyper-
tension.   Also not  fully  resolved was the extent  to which  lead-induced hyper-
tension or other  types of lead-induced pathogenic effects contribute to more
serious morbidity (heart  attack,  stroke)  and mortality of  the human population.
     For the purposes of  this  supplement, primary  emphasis will be placed here
on  updated  discussion of  key  human population-based  studies  which  reflect
primarily non-occupational  exposure to lead.  The  largest study  populations
have been the  second  National  Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES
II)  for  the U.S. population  (performed  during the years  1976-80)  and the
British Regional  Heart Study (BRHS), an ongoing  evaluation  of men aged 40  to  59
from 24 British  towns.   These  studies were earlier described in detail  in the
published literature  (Harlan et al.,  1985;  Pocock  et  al., 1984; Pirkle  et al.,
1985; Shaper et  al.,  1981); and further  analyses  of the subject data  sets have
been presented  and  discussed  extensively  at a 1987  U.S.  EPA  co-sponsored
International  Symposium  on Lead-Blood Pressure  Relationships  and in other
recent publications.  In  addition,  analyses  of  several other  data sets have
been presented by other investigators at  the 1987  Symposium or elsewhere in  the
published literature since the 1986 Addendum was  prepared.
     The 1986  Addendum  (U.S.   EPA,  1986b) noted that  several  then  recently
published studies provided  generally  consistent  evidence for  increased blood
pressure (BP)  being associated with  elevated lead body burdens  in  adults,
especially as  indexed by  blood lead (PbB)  levels  in  various cohorts  of adult
men.  None of the individual studies, it  was noted, provided definitive evidence
establishing causal  relationships between lead exposure and increased blood
pressure, but  they  collectively  provided considerable qualitative  evidence
indicative of  significant  associations between  blood  lead and blood pressure
3/27/89

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


levels.  It was further emphasized that estimates of quantitative relationships
between blood  lead  levels  and blood pressure  increases  derived  from  such  study
results are  subject to much  uncertainty,  given the relatively  small  sample
sizes  and  limited  population  groups typically studied.   On  the  other hand,  the
above-noted  larger-scale  studies  (NHANES II and BRHS)  of  general  population
groups, were  singled  out  as providing reasonably good bases for estimation of
quantitative blood-lead blood-pressure relationships.
     As reviewed in the  1986  Addendum (U.S.  EPA,  1986b),  in  the BRHS,  Pocock
et al.   (1984) evaluated  relationships  between  blood lead concentrations,
hypertension,  and  renal  function  indicators  in a clinical survey  of 7,735
middle-aged men (aged 40-49) from 24 British towns.  Each man's blood pressure,
while  seated,  was  measured twice  in succession by means of a  London School  of
Hygiene sphygmomanometer.    Diastolic  pressure was  recorded at  phase  V  disap-
pearance of  sounds.   The  mean of the  two readings of  blood pressure was
adjusted for observed variation within each town to correct for any differences
among  three  observers.  Results  for  7,371 men included in data analyses indi-
cated  correlation  coefficients of r  = +0.03  and r = +0.01 for  associations
between systolic and  diastolic blood  pressure,  respectively, and  blood lead
levels.  The  systolic  blood pressure  correlation,  though small  in magnitude,
was  nevertheless statistically significant at p <0.01.   However, analyses  of
covariance using data  for  men categorized according to  blood lead concentra-
tions only suggested increases in blood pressure at lower blood lead levels; no
further significant increments in  blood  pressure were observed  at higher  blood
lead levels  either  before  or  after adjustment for factors  such as age, town,
body mass  index, alcohol  consumption, social  class,  and observer.   Evaluation
of prevalence of hypertension  defined  as systolic blood pressure over 160  mm Hg
revealed no  significant overall trend; but of those men  with  blood lead levels
over 37 ug/dl, a larger proportion (30 percent)  had hypertension when compared
with the proportion (21 percent)  for all  other men combined (p =0.08).  Similar
results were obtained for diastolic hypertension defined as >100 mm Hg, i.e.,  a
greater proportion  (15 percent) of men with blood lead levels  over  37 ug/dl  had
diastolic  hypertension in  comparison  with the proportion (9  percent)  for all
other men  (p =0.07).
     Pocock  et al.   (1984)  interpreted their findings as being  suggestive of
increased  hypertension at  blood  lead  levels over  37 ug/dl, but not  at lower
concentrations typically  found in British men.    However,  further  analyses
3/27/89

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE  OR  CITE


reported by Pocock  et  al.  (1985)  for the same data  indicated highly statisti-
cally significant associations between  both  systolic  (p =0.003)  and diastolic
(p <0.001) blood pressure and  blood  lead  levels, when adjustments are made for
variation due to site  (town)  in  multiple regression analyses.  The  regression
coefficients for log blood  lead  versus systolic and diastolic pressure  were
+2.089 and +1.809,  respectively,  when  adjusted for  town as  well  as  body  mass,
age, alcohol, smoking,  social  class  and observer.   Noting the small magnitude
of the association observed and the difficulty in adjusting  for all  potentially
relevant confounders,  Pocock  et  al  (1985) cautioned  at that time  against
prematurely concluding that elevated body lead burden  has a  causal  influence  on
blood pressure.
     The 1986 Addendum  (U.S.  EPA,  1986b)  also  noted that relationships between
blood lead and  blood  pressure among American adults had begun to be evaluated
in  another  large-scale study, as  reported by Harlan et al.  (1985), Pirkle
et al.  (1985),  and  Schwartz  (1985a,b;  1986a,b).   These analyses  were based on
evaluation of  NHANES  II data,  which provide  careful  blood lead and blood
pressure measurements  on a large-scale sample  representative of  the U.S.
population and  considerable  information  on  a wide variety of  potentially
confounding variables  as well.  As such,  these analyses avoided  the problem  of
selection bias, the healthy-worker effect,  workplace  exposures to other  toxic
agents,  and  problems  with appropriate  choice of control groups that often
confounded or complicated  earlier,  occupational  studies of  blood-lead blood-
pressure relationships.  Three blood pressure readings were recorded for each
subject:   while seated  early  in  the  examination,  supine midway in the examina-
tion, and  seated  near  the  end.  First  and  fifth phase sounds were  taken  as
systolic  and  diastolic  pressures,  respectively.  The  second seated blood
pressure was  used  in  statistical  analyses,  but analyses using the first  seated
pressure  or  a mean of the first  and second seated pressure yielded similar
results.   Blood lead values, determined by AA spectrometry,  were  transformed to
log values used in statistical analyses.
     Relationships between blood pressure and other variables were evaluated in
two  ways.   First,  men  and  women were stratified into  normotensive and
hypertensive  categories  and mean  values for  relevant  variables contrasted
across the  categories.   For  ages 21-55 yr, diastolic high blood pressure (>90
mm  Hg) male  subjects  (N =  475) had significantly (p <0.005) higher  PbB  levels,
body mass  index values,  and calcium foods than did normotensive male subjects

3/27/89                              4

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


(N = 1,043).   Similar results were  obtained  for aged 21-55 yr diastolic high
blood  pressure  females  (N  = 263)   in  comparison  to  normotensive females
(N = 1,316).   For ages  56-74 yr subjects, significantly (p <0.05) higher PbB
levels were  found for  female  subjects (but  not males) defined  as  having
isolated systolic high blood pressure (i.e.,  systolic >160  and diastolic <90 mm
Hg).  Simple correlation  analyses  and step-wise multiple  regression analyses
were carried out  as  a second statistical  evaluation approach; PbB values were
entered into predictive  models  for systolic  and diastolic  pressure as  well  as
several other pertinent  variables  (such as age,  body  mass  index,  etc.)  entered
sequentially according  to greatest magnitude  of variance  explained for the
dependent variable.    The  simple  correlation  analyses  reported by  Harlan et  al.
(1985) demonstrated  statistically  significant linear associations (p <0.001)
between blood  lead  concentrations  and blood  pressure  (both systolic  and
diastolic) among  males  and  females,  aged 12  to 74 years.   Using multiple
regression analyses  controlling  for a number of other potentially confounding
factors,   however,  the   blood-lead  blood-pressure  associations  remained
significant for males but not for  women  after  adjusting  for the effects of
other pertinent variables.
     Additional  analyses  of  NHANES  II data reported  by Pirkle  et al.  (1985)
focussed on white males  (aged 40 to 59 years)  in order  to  avoid the effects  of
collinearity between  blood  pressure and  blood lead concentrations evident  at
earlier ages and  because of less extensive NHANES II data  being available for
non-whites.   In the  subgroup studied, Pirkle et al.  (1985) found significant
associations between blood   lead and  blood pressure even after including in
multiple regression  analyses  all known factors  previously  established as being
correlated with blood pressure.   The relationship also held when tested against
every  dietary  and  serologic variable  measured  in the NHANES II  study.
Inclusion of  both curvilinear transformations and  interaction  terms  altered
little the  coefficients  for blood pressure associations  with  lead  (the
strongest relationship was  observed between  the natural log of blood lead and
the blood pressure measures). The  regression coefficients  for  log blood lead
versus  systolic   and diastolic  blood  pressure  were 8.436  and  3.954,
respectively.   No evident threshold was  found  below which  blood lead level  was
not significantly related to blood pressure  across a range of  7  to 34  ug/dl.
In fact, the dose-response  relationships  characterized  by  Pirkle  et al.  (1985)
indicate that large  initial  increments in blood  pressure  occur at relatively

3/27/89                              5

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


low blood lead levels, followed by leveling off of blood pressure increments  at
higher blood  lead levels.   Pirkle  et al.  (1985)  also  found lead to  be  a
significant predictor  of diastolic blood  pressure  greater than or equal  to
90 mmHg,  the  criterion blood pressure  level  now standardly employed in the
United States to  define  hypertension.   Additional analyses were performed by
Pirkle et al.  (1985) to estimate the likely public health implications  of their
findings  concerning blood-lead, blood-pressure relationships.   Changes  in blood
pressure  that might  result  from a specified change in  blood lead  levels were
first  estimated.   Then coefficients from the Pooling Project  and  Framingham
studies  (Pooling  Project Research  Group,  1978 and McGee  and  Gordon,  1976,
respectively)  of  cardiovascular  disease were used as bases:   (1)  to estimate
the risk for incidence of serious cardiovascular events  (myocardial infarction,
stroke, or death) as a consequence of lead-induced blood pressure increases and
(2) to predict the  change in the number of serious  outcomes as  the result  of a
37 percent  decrease  in blood lead  levels  for  adult white males (aged 40-59
years) observed during the course of the NHANES II survey (1976-1980).
     The  earlier  Addendum (U.S.  EPA,  1986b)  also noted  that questions  had  been
raised by  Gartside (1985) and E.I. Du  Pont  de Nemours  (1986)  regarding the
robustness  of the  findings  derived from  the  analyses  of  NHANES  II  data
discussed above and as to whether certain time trends in the NHANES II  data set
may have  contributed to (or account for) the reported blood-lead blood-pressure
relationships.  Gartside  reported  analyses of HNANES II data which found that
the size and  level  of statistical  significance  of coefficients  obtained  varied
depending  upon  specific  data  aggregations used in analyzing the  data.  The
largest and most  significant coefficients  for  blood lead  versus blood  pressure
were obtained by  Gartside for data aggregated by age groups that approximated
that of  the 40-59 yr male aggregation described by Pirkle et al. (1985), with
coefficients  for  most younger cohort groups aggregated by  varying 20  yr age
intervals  (e.g.,  21-40,  22-42  yrs, etc.)  or older groups  not always  being
significant at p <0.05.  As for the time trend issue, both blood lead and blood
pressure declined  substantially  during the 4-yr NHANES II study and different
geographic  sites  were sampled without  revisitation of  the  same site over  the
survey period.  Thus,  variations in the sampling  sites over  time, coincident
with  changes  in  blood  lead and/or blood  pressure, might contribute  to any
observed associations  between blood lead and blood pressure.    E.I. Du Pont de
Nemours  (1986)  reported  that multiple  regression  coefficients  decreased  in
magnitude  and some became non-significant at p  <0.05 when geographic site was
3/27/89                              6

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
adjusted for  in  analyses  of NHANES II data,  including  analyses  for the male
group  (aged  12-74)  reported on by Harlan  et  al.  (1985) and for males  (aged
40-59)  reported  on by  Pirkle  et  al.  (1985).   For example, E.I. Du Pont de
Nemours reported  unpublished  reanalyses  of NHANES II data confirming signifi-
cant associations for  both  aged 12-74 yrs  males  and  40-59  yr males  between  log
PbB and systolic  or diastolic blood pressure unadjusted  for geographic site,
but smaller  coefficients  (nonsignificant for  diastolic) when geographic  site
was included  in  the analysis.   However,  neither the Gartside nor E.I.  Du Pont
de Nemours analyses  adjusted  for  all  of the  variables  that were selected for
stepwise inclusion  in the  Harlan et al.  (1985)  and Pirkle et al.  (1985)
published analyses  by  means of a priori  decision rules for inclusion of vari-
ables having significant associations  with blood pressure.   Also,  other differ-
ences existed in regard to specific aspects of the modeling approaches employed,
making  it extremely  difficult to  assess  clearly  the  potential impact of varia-
tion in selection  of age  groups  and geographic  site adjustment  on NHANES II
analyses results.
     In order to address the "site" issue more definitively, Schwartz (1985a,b;
1986a,b) carried  out  a series of additional reanalyses of the NHANES II data.
Those  analyses confirm that the regression coefficients remain significant  for
both systolic and  diastolic blood pressure when  site is included as a  variable
in multiple  regression analyses.   Of  several different  approaches used by
Schwartz,  the most direct was holding all aspects of the original  Pirkle et al.
(1985) analyses  the  same  except for the  addition of  a variable controlling  for
the 64  geographic  sites sampled in NHANES  II.  Using this  approach, the coffi-
cients  for  log  PbB  in relation  to either diastolic  or systolic  BP dropped
somewhat from  those of the original  analyses when  site  was  controlled for
(i.e.,  from 8.44 to 5.09 for systolic  and from 3.95 to 2.74 for diastolic blood
pressure),  but the coefficients for each still remained significant at p <0.05.
When still  other approaches were used to control  for site along with variations
in other variables  included in the analyses,  statistically significant results
were still  consistently obtained  both  for males aged 40-59 and for males aged
20-74.   The  results  obtained  by Schwartz (1985a,b; 1986a,b) via reanalysis  of
NHANES  II data  (unadjusted  versus adjusted for geographic site) are presented
in Table 1 in comparison  to results reported  by  E.I.  Du Pont de Nemours (1986)
and in  relation  to  the findings  presented  by  Pocock et al.  (1984, 1985) for
British men (also unadjusted versus adjusted for site).
3/27/89

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
    TABLE 1.   COEFFICIENTS FOR THE NATURAL LOG OF BLOOD LEAD CONCENTRATION
           (logPbB) VS.  BLOOD PRESSURE (BP) IN MEN WITH AND WITHOUT
                         ADJUSTMENT FOR SITE VARIABLES
  Analysis
Performed by
       Study
       Group
                                                          Coefficient of
                                                          log PbB vs.  BP
Unadjusted
 for Site
Adjusted
for Site
Pocock et al.
  (1984, 1985)
Schwartz (1985a,b)
E.I.  Du Pont
  de Nemours(1986)
Schwartz (1986a,b)
E.I.  Du Pont
  de Nemours (1986)
British Regiona]
 Heart Study
White males aged 40-59
     Systolic (n=7371)
     Diastolic (n=7371)

NHANES II
Males aged 20-74
     Systolic (n=2254)
     Diastolic (n=2248)

NHANES II
Males aged 12-74
     Systolic (n=2794)
     Diastolic (n=2789)

NHANES II
White males aged 40-59
     Systolic (n=543)
     Diastolic (n=565)

NHANES II
White males aged 40-59
     Systolic (n=553)
     Diastolic (n=575)
                                                     1.68**
                                                     0.30
                                                     5.23***
                                                     2.96***
                                                     3.43***
                                                     2.02***
                                                     8.44**
                                                     3.95**
                                                     6.27**
                                                     4.01**
                  2.09**
                  1.81***
                  3.23**
                  1.39*
                  1.95*
                  0.36
                  5.01*
                  2.74*
                  3.46*
                  1.93*
  *p < 0.05
 **p < 0.01
***p < 0.001


     The 1986 Addendum  (U.S.  EPA,  1986b)  concluded  that,  overall,  the  analyses

of data  from  the  two large-scale general  population studies (British Regional

Heart  Study  and U.S. NHANES  II  Study)  discussed above collectively provide

highly  convincing  evidence  demonstrating  small but statistically  significant

associations between  blood  lead  levels  and increased blood  pressure  in  adult

men.   The  strongest  associations appear to exist for males aged 40-59 and for

systolic somewhat  more  so  than for diastolic  pressure.   Virtually all of the

analyses revealed  positive  associations for the  40-59  aged group,  which  remain

or become  significant (at  p <0.05) when adjustments are  made  for geographic

site.   Furthermore,  the  results  of these large-scale studies were noted to be
3/27/89
              8

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
consistent with  similar findings  of  statistically significant  associations
between blood  lead  levels  and blood pressure increases  as  derived  from  other
smaller-scale  studies  discussed in the  Addendum, which  also mainly  found
stronger associations for systolic pressure than for diastolic.
     The Addendum (U.S.  EPA, 1986b) further concluded that none of the observa-
tional studies in  and  of themselves can  be stated as  definitively establishing
causal linkages  between  lead exposure and  increased  blood  pressure of hyper-
tension.   However,  the  Addendum noted that the plausibility  of  the observed
associations reflecting  causal  relationships  between  lead exposure  and  blood
pressure increases  is  supported by:   (1) the consistency of  the significant
associations found by numerous independent investigators for a variety of study
populations; and (2) by extensive toxicological  data discussed  in  the  1986
Addendum which clearly demonstrate increases  in  blood  pressure for animal
models under well-controlled experimental  conditions.  The precise mechanisms
underlying relationships between  lead exposure  and increased blood pressure,
the Addendum  further stated,  appear  to be complex and  mathematical models
describing the relationships still  remained to be more  definitively charac-
terized.   At the time  of the Addendum log  PbB-BP models  appeared to fit best
the available  data, but linear relationships between blood  lead and blood
pressure could not  be  ruled out.   The most appropriate  coefficients character-
izing  PbB-BP  relationships  also  remained  to be  more precisely determined,
although those listed  in  Table 1  obtained by  analyses adjusting  for site
appeared to be the  currently best available and  most reasonable estimates of
the likely strength  of  the association for adult men (i.e.,  generally in the
range  of 2.0-5.0 for log PbB versus systolic and  1.4  to  2.7 for  log PbB  versus
diastolic blood  pressure).  The 1986 Addendum went on to note that  the  full
range of blood lead levels  that may be associated with increased blood pressure
also remained  to be more clearly defined.   However,  the  collective evidence
from the  above studies points  toward  low  or moderately elevated blood  lead
levels as being associated  with blood  pressure increases, with certain evidence
(e.g., the NHANES II data  analyses and some other study  results) also indicat-
ing significant  relationships  between blood pressure  elevations  and blood lead
levels ranging down, possibly, to as  low as 7 (jg/dl.,
     The earlier  Addendum (U.S. EPA,  1986b) further stated that the quantifica-
tion  of  likely  consequent risks for  serious  cardiovascular  outcomes,  as
attempted by   Pirkle et al.   (1985),  also  remained  to be  more  precisely

3/27/89                             9

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
characterized.   The specific magnitudes  of  risk obtained for serious cardio-
vascular outcomes  in  relation to  lead  exposure,  estimated on the  basis  of
lead-induced blood pressure increases, depend crucially upon:  the  form of  the
underlying relationship and size  of  the coefficients  estimated  for blood-lead
blood-pressure associations; lead  exposure  levels  at  which significant  eleva-
tions  in  blood pressure  occur;  and  coefficients  estimating relationships
between blood  pressure increases  and  specific  more  serious  cardiovascular
outcomes.   It  was  noted  that  uncertainty still existed regarding  the  most
appropriate model  and blood-lead blood-pressure coefficients, which  made it
difficult to resolve  which  specific  coefficients  should be used in attempting
to project more serious  cardiovascular  outcomes.   Similarly,  it was indicated
that it is difficult  to  determine appropriate blood  lead  levels at which any
selected coefficients might be appropriately applied  in models predicting more
serious cardiovascular outcomes.   Lastly,  it  was  noted that  the selection  of
appropriate models and coefficients  relating  blood pressure increases to  more
serious outcomes is also  fraught with uncertainty.  Questions exist regarding
the general applicability of coefficients derived  from the  Pooling  Projects  and
Framingham Study to the men aged 40-59 in the  general  U.S.  population.   Further
analyses of additional large  scale epidemiologic  data sets,  it was stated  in
the  Addendum,  would be  necessary  to determine more  precisely  quantitative
relationships between blood-lead  and  blood-pressure,  and more serious cardio-
vascular outcomes as well.
     The Addendum  (U.S.  EPA,  1986b)  went on to note  further that  the  above
findings,  while pointing  toward  a  likely causal effect  of  lead  in  contributing
to increased blood  pressure need to  be placed in  broader perspective in rela-
tion to other factors involved in the etiology of  hypertension.   The underlying
causes of  increased blood pressure or  "hypertension"  (diastolic  blood pressure
above 90 mm Hg), which  occurs  in as many as 25 percent  of Americans, are not
yet fully delineated (Frohlich, 1983; Kaplan,  1983).  However, it is very clear
that many  factors  contribute to  development of  this disease,  including  heredi-
tary traits, nutritional  factors  and  environmental  agents.  The  relative  roles
of various dietary  and environmental  factors  in influencing blood  pressure  and
the mechanisms by which they do so are a matter of intense  investigative effort
and  debate  (see proceedings of  conference "Nutrition  and Blood  Pressure:
Current Status  of Dietary  Factors and Hypertension," McCarron  and Kotchen,
1983).   The contribution  of lead, compared to many other factors evaluated  in

3/27/89                              10

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
various analyses discussed  above,  appears to be relatively small, usually not
accounting for more  than  1-2 percent of the variation explained by the models
employed when other significant factors are controlled for in the analyses.
     Many of the above  issues and questions have been further addressed since
the  1986  Addendum  was  completed,  in part  at the 1987  Lead-Blood  Pressure
Symposium and  in other  recent publications as well.   Presentations  and dis-
cussions at the  1987 Symposium (see Victery, 1988a)  were  contributed by (1)
many of the key scientists who have carried out important investigations of the
relationship of  lead  exposure and  hypertension (including  observational,
epidemiologic,  and  experimental  reports),  as well  as (2)  other experts in
hypertension and cardiovascular disease  in  general.   Several  speakers reviewed
their analyses of  the  NHANES II  data (Marian, 1988; Schwartz, 1988; Gartside,
1988; Landis and Flegal,  1988) and the  British  study (Pocock et al., 1988).
Elwood et al.   (1988)  also summarized the results of two new large-scale Welsh
studies.   Pocock further provided a new comparison of the relative magnitude of
the  contribution of  lead  exposure to blood pressure  changes  based on inter-
comparison of  the  results of  the  NHANES II,  British, and Welsh  studies.   A
number of other  relevant  papers  were also  presented  which  described studies
of lead-blood  pressure  relationships  in  selected populations  of  nonoccupation-
ally exposed individuals,  as well  as workers (Kromhout, 1988; Moreau et al.,
1988; Weiss et al.,  1988;  Wedeen,  1988;  Cooper,  1988;  Selevan et al., 1988;  de
Kort and  Zwennis,  1988;  Elwood  et al. ,   1988;  Neri  et al. ,  1988;  Staessen
et al.,  1988;  Sharp et al., 1988).  Other  presentations  addressed pertinent
toxicological   findings  from  HI vivo or J_n vitro animal studies (Vander, 1988;
Chai  and Webb, 1988;  Kopp et al., 1988;  Boscolo  and  Carmignani,  1988;  Weiler
et al.,  1988;  Victery, 1988b).
     With regard to  new findings  reported at  the Symposium,  a further alterna-
tive  analysis  of the data from NHANES II (use of a generalized Mantel-Haenzel
test) was reported  by  Landis and  Flegal  (1988).   In order  to  more definitively
assess the robustness of  the earlier NHANES  II  results  (Harlan  et al.  1985;
Pirkle et al.,  1985;  etc)  and, also, to  evaluate possible  time-trend effects
confounded by  variations  in  sampling sites,  Landis and  Flegal  (1988) carried
out  further analyses  for  NHANES  II males,  aged  12-74,  using  a randomization
model-based approach  to test  the  statistical significance  of the  partial
correlation between blood  lead and diastolic blood pressure, adjusting for age,
body  mass  index,  and the 64  NHANES  II  sampling sites.   Simple  linear  and
multiple regression coefficients between log PbB and diastolic BP for all males
3/27/89                              11

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
(aged 12-74) were 0.15 and 4.90,  respectively;  for various  groups  broken out by
age (<20, 21-39, >40  yrs)  and body mass  index levels, the respective coeffi-
cients ranged from  0.04  to 0.12  and from 1.29 to 3.55 (predominantly between
2.3 and 3.6), displaying considerable consistency across  age-body  mass  compari-
son groups.   Also,  the  most  stringent or  "conservative"  approach used to
calculate a  randomized model  statistic  controlling for effects  due to 64
sampling sites yielded a test  statistic of 4.62 (still  significant at p <0.05).
The authors  noted that:   (1)  the association must  be  sufficiently robust  to
persist across  478  subgroups  formed on the  basis  of  factors also having  an
association with the levels of blood lead and blood pressure; and  (2) neverthe-
less,  even with  the severe adjustments by sampling sites, age,  and Quetelet
index, the diastolic  blood pressure/blood lead relationship  remained statisti-
cally significant at  the p <0.05 level.   In summary, their  analyses indicate
that the significant linear association between blood-lead  levels  and diastolic
blood pressure readings cannot be dismissed as due to concurrent secular trends
in  the  two  variables across the  4-year  survey period.  Finally, the authors
noted that,  even though these  partial correlations are not  large,  the magnitude
of the regression coefficients suggests  that elevated blood  lead  levels  may  be
an important  risk factor for  elevated blood pressures,  as developed in con-
siderably greater detail  for  the restricted group of white men 40 to 59 years
of age (as described in Pirkle et al.,  1985).
     Also reported  on  at the  1988 Symposium were  two  surveys in  Wales which
evaluated possible  relationships  between  blood lead and  blood pressure  (Elwood
et al., 1988a,b).  The Welsh Heart Programme was  carried  out  in 1985 throughout
Wales, using a  stratified  cluster  random  sample of 21,000  households,  with
2,010 male and  female adult subjects being  included  in  the  survey population
for whom  blood  lead  determinations  were made.  Complete  blood  lead,  blood
pressure, and other key  data  were available for  865  men and 856  women.  Mean
blood  lead values for men  were 12.4  |jg/dl and  for  women  9.6  ug/dl.   Blood  lead
increased with  age  (1 |jg/dl every 15.6 years  in men and every 13.1 years  in
women).  The  partial  regression  coefficients with  standard error  were  reported
after the effect of age had been removed by cubic regression.  In males,  the
coefficients  for systolic  and diastolic pressure were 0.82  + 1.49 and 1.29 +
0.95,  respectively.    For females, these  coefficients  were  0.19 +  1.46  and  0.54
+ 1.00.   In  neither sex was the relationship statistically significant. Cate-
gorizing the  data by blood lead increments demonstrated no trend  in the blood
pressure levels.
3/27/89                              12

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     The second  group  studied  was about half of  the  planned subjects in the
Caerphilly  Collaborative  Heart Disease  study;  this  study on ischemic  heart
disease was based  on  a cohort  of  men  aged 45-59,  living  in Caerphilly,  a  small
town in Wales. Several risk factors for ischemic heart disease were considered.
Blood pressure was measured with a random-zero muddler sphygmomanometer after a
5-minute rest and  during  a cold pressor test.  The  correlation coefficients
for resting systolic  and  diastolic pressure were 0.0183  and 0.0230,  respec-
tively, and for  the  rise  in pressure with  the  cold  pressor test, 0.0342 and
0.0078  respectively.   None were  significant  at p <0.05.  Ranking the  blood
pressure readings  according  to blood  lead groups did  not reveal  any  trend  in
the percentage of subjects with systolic blood pressure greater than 160 mm Hg.
     The authors (Elwood  et  al.,  1988a,b) only  corrected for age  as the single
confounding factor controlled  for, because  all other  factors are positively
correlated with  both  blood pressure and blood  lead.   They concluded  that,  in
the event  these  other factors  (e.g.,  cigarette use  and  alcohol consumption)
were controlled  for,   they would  probably  reduce  the already  trivial  (and
nonsignificant)  relationships  found by  them for blood lead  and blood pressure
measurements in the two Welsh studies.
     At the 1988 Symposium,  Pocock compared the results of  the NHANES II data
analyses as discussed  by  Schwartz (1988),  his  analyses  of  the  British study
(Pocock et al.,  1988), and  the two Welsh  studies (Elwood et al.,  1988a,b).
Figure 1 shows the magnitudes  of  effects obtained for adult men in these four
large-scale studies,   relating  systolic  blood  pressure  to blood  lead concentra-
tion,  as  depicted  by  Pocock et al.   (1988).   The graph  shows  each study's
estimated  change in mean  systolic blood pressure for  each  doubling of blood
lead (e.g., from 8 to 16 ug/dl),  together with 95% confidence limits.  Pocock
concluded  that an  overview of  data from these  large  epidemiological  surveys
provide reasonably consistent  evidence  on  lead and blood pressure.   That is,
whereas the  NHANES II data  on 2,254  U.S.  men  indicate  a slightly stronger
association between  blood lead and  systolic blood  pressure than  Pocock1s
British study,  the data from  the  two Welsh studies on  over 2,000 men also
showed  a  small  positive,  (but not statistically  significant)  association.
Pocock  noted  that  the  overlaping confidence limits  for all these studies
suggest that  there may be a weak positive statistical  association whereby
systolic blood pressure in adult  men  is increased by  about  1 mm  Hg for every
doubling of blood lead concentration.

3/27/89                              13

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
                                                          BRHS(N=7371)

                                                          NHANE S II (N=2254)

                                                          Caerphilly (N=1164)

                                                          Wales (N=865)
 -2-101        234
      Estimated change in mean systolic blood pressure
             (mm Hg) for a doubling of blood lead
Figure 1.  Comparison of study results from four larger-scale epidemiology
          studies of lead-blood pressure  relationships  in audit men.  BRHS =
          British Regional  Heart  Study analyses, described by  Pocock et al.
          (1988); NHANES II = National  Health  and Nutrition Evaluation Survey
          analyses described by Schwartz  (1988);  Caerphilly and Wales = Welsh
          studies described by Elwood et al.  (1988a,b).
Source:   Pocock et al.  (1988).

     There was an additional report of an analysis of cross-sectional data from
Canada (Neri et al., 1988) presented at the 1988 Lead-Blood Pressure Symposium.
This study  was  performed  on data collected during  10 months  of 1978-1979 for
2,193 subjects  aged  25 to 64.   The  zero-order  correlation between diastolic
blood pressure  and  blood  lead level was  found  to be 0.115.   The authors con-
cluded that,  although  this association is a weak one (as was also the case in
the NHANES  II data),  its  statistical significance is not in doubt (p <0.001).
They further concluded that (1) the Canadian data were at least weakly support-
ive of the  inference drawn from NHANES II, in  that elevation of blood lead did
seem  to  entail  some risk  of  blood pressure elevation,  but  (2) it would be
3/27/89
14

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


premature, in  the  absence  of longitudinal  data,  to  infer  that  this  is  a  cause-
and-effect relationship.   Neri  et al.,  1988 then reported the  findings  for  a
group  of  subjects  studied  longitudinally  for blood  lead levels and  blood
pressure.   That  is,  in a study of  lead  foundry  workers,  an association was
found  between  short-term changes  in an individual's  blood  lead level  and
contemporary changes  in  diastolic pressure, which remained significant  after
allowance for  age  (or time) trends and for effects attributable to  changes in
body weight.   The authors also noted that short-term changes  in urinary cadmium
were similarly predictive of diastolic blood pressure levels.
     There were  several  other  reports presented at the 1988  Symposium  with
regard to smaller-scale  observational  studies  of blood  lead and blood  pressure
in two types of occupationally lead-exposed groups:   bus drivers (Sharp et al.,
1988)  and  policemen  (Weiss  et  al.,  1988  and  Moreau et al.,  1988).   These
studies all  yielded findings that are also indicative of lead-induced increases
in blood pressure.
     In other  recent  reports besides those presented at  the 1988 Symposium,
three  new  studies  (with generally  small  study  cohorts)  of occupationally
exposed workers  have  yielded further results with regard  to  lead effects on
blood pressure or other cardiovascular outcomes.   For example,  Parkinson et al.
(1987) reported  findings on data  collected in 1982  from  270 lead workers  and
158 nonexposed workers  in  Pennsylvania.   Four measures of lead exposure were
used:   (a) employment at lead exposure vs. control  plants;  (b) current  blood
lead values  at time  of  examination (exposed  group  mean  =  39.9 ug/dl  vs.
7.4 pg/dl  for controls); (c) zinc protoporphyrin  values at time of examination:
(d) time  weighted  average  (TWA)  blood  lead values for lead-exposed workers
since date of  hire.   Other risk  factors (age, years  of education,  etc.) were
also included  in the  regression analysis.   Of the three  lead  measures,  only
TWA blood  lead was significantly,  although modestly, correlated with blood
pressure.   After controlling for  other predictive risk  factors, including  age,
the effect of  TWA  on  blood pressure was no longer significant  and the authors
did not find evidence  of renal disease.
     Also,  in  1987, de  Kort and  colleagues reported  finding a statistically
significant increase  in  systolic  and diastolic pressure  in 53 exposed males
compared with  52 controls  (de Kort  et al,  1987).   Blood  lead  averaged  47.4
ug/dl  vs.  8.1  for  controls.   Blood and urine cadmium were also higher than in
controls;  there were  no  adverse effects  on kidney function.  The prevalence  of
clinically-defined   hypertension  (systolic  greater than  160  mm  Hg and/or
3/27/89                              15

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
diastolic greater than  95  mm Hg and/or under treatment for hypertension) was
higher in the exposed group,  but the observed relative  risk was  not different.
     In addition to  the above study results which focussed primarily on lead-
blood pressure effects  in  adult males, some additional  analyses of such rela-
tionships in females have become available since the 1986  Addendum and the 1987
Lead-Blood  Pressure  Symposium.   For example,  Schwartz  (1989)  has recently
reported the  results of still  further analyses of the NHANES  II  data set.
These latest analyses  used variables  identified as being  important in earlier
NHANES II analyses and  examined the relationship between  blood  lead and blood
pressure in  both males  and  females,   aged  20-74 years.   The analyses  were
carried out  in stages, with  separate  regressions first being performed  for
males and  females.   Diastolic  blood  pressure was  regressed  on age,  age2,
Quetelet's body mass  index,  and the natural  log  of  blood lead.   If, in this
first step,  lead was significant,  then a stepwise  regression was  carried out
taking into account a number  of covariates or potentially  confounding cofactors
(e.g., dietary sodium,  smoking,  etc.); and final models  were then ultimately
estimated using  SURREGR (a  program correcting for study  design  effects  as
well).  Statistically  significant  relationships  were  found between blood lead
and blood pressure  in  both males (p <0.01) and females  (p <0.01) in the first
stage  regressions,   correcting  for the above-noted  covariates.    Lead  also
continued to be  significantly related  to blood pressure in the  separate step-
wise  regressions for males and females.   The final models yielded a smaller,
but statistically  significant  (p <0.03)  blood  lead coefficient  (1.640)  for
females than  the coefficient  for  males  (2.928;  p <0.006).   These  results
therefore (1)  further  confirm  the earlier  findings  of Schwartz  and  other
investigators  demonstrating  significant associations between blood lead and
blood pressure increases  in  adult  males in the NHANES II  study  population and
(2) demonstrate  analogous, although somewhat smaller effects  in women from  the
same study population.
     In another  study, Grandjean  et al.  (1989)  evaluated lead-hypertension
relationships  in a  cohort  of 504  men  and  548 women born  in the same year and
residing in  the  Glostrup area of Denmark.  Both blood lead concentrations and
blood pressure determinations were first obtained at age 40  and then, again,
five  years  later  for 451 men  and  410  of  the women.  The  average  blood lead
levels for  men at  age  40 and 45 years were 13 and 9 (jg/dl, respectively;  for
women they  were  9  and  6 ug/dl, respectively,  at age 40 and 45  years.   At age
40, women with systolic blood pressure above 140 mm Hg and/or diastolic above
3/27/89                              16

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
90 mm  Hg  had  slightly  elevated  blood  leads  compared to  non-hypertensive women;
but  no differences were found  for  blood lead levels in men  with increased
versus normal  (i.e., 140 or 90 mm Hg,  respectively,  for systolic and diastolic)
blood pressure.   Significant correlations were found between log blood lead and
systolic blood pressure  in  both men and women and diastolic blood pressure in
women  only at age  40,  but not  at age 45.   The relationship at aged 40 indi-
cated  a doubling of blood  lead being associated with  a <3 mm Hg increase in
blood pressure.   When blood hemoglobin and alcohol  intake (the only two of nine
potential  confounders  assessed  that were significantly related to  both blood
lead and blood pressure) were entered into multiple regression analyses,  then
all blood-lead blood-pressure associations  become  non-significant.   Grandjean
et al.  (1989) noted that the initial  association found between blood lead and
blood  pressure was  similar  or slightly weaker than  those earlier reported for
the  BRHS  (Pocock et al., 1985) or the  NHANES II  (Pirkle et al., 1985) data
analyses.   The impact  of alcohol  and  blood hemoglobin  on  the  subsequent  mul-
tiple  regression outcomes  indicate  clear confounding with blood  lead  and an
inability in this  study  to separate out relative contributions  due to those
factors versus lead (with blood lead increasing with number of units of alcohol
consumed as documented in  the  study  cohort  at  age  40, for example).  The
authors (Grandjean  et  al.)  further noted that non-response and loss  to follow-
up, the very  low range of  blood lead  values,  and the limited  statistical  power
of the study  (with fewer  than  1,000  subjects) to detect  small  effects  may
help to account  for their observed pattern of results.
     The issue of lead effects on more serious cardiovascular disease outcomes,
possibly mediated by  lead  effects  in  blood pressure or via other lead-induced
pathogenic processes has been further addressed  since  the  1986 Addendum to  the
EPA Lead Criteria  Document  was  prepared.  Some of the  newer evidence has  been
derived from studies  of  occupationally  exposed workers and other new  results
have emerged  from   still further  analyses  of the two  larger-scale  BRHS  and
NHANES II  data sets.
     The causes  of  mortality of lead workers in the  United Kingdom between 1926
and  1985,  for example,  have  recently been updated in  a  case-control  study
written by Fanning  (1988).   There  were 867 deaths in men with relatively high
occupational  exposure  to lead  during  these years, and  1206 deaths  during the
same period in men  whose exposure had been  low  or absent.  During  the period
between 1946 and 1965  there was a significant excess of deaths from cardiovas-
cular disease; but  there was no difference between the  two groups over the past
3/27/89                              17

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
20 years. Also,  no  statistically significant excess in the  number  of deaths
from malignant  neoplasms  was evident.   Fanning  concluded that the previous
evidence of an  increased  risk of death from cerebrovascular disease  (in  lead
workers) was therefore  confirmed, but  it would seem that with the introduction
of stricter standards  of  lead control  this  increased risk has now disappeared,
as has any marginal  risk of death from malignant  disease.
     Another occupational exposure  study,  an analysis  of causes of death in
U.S.  battery and lead  production workers during the years  1947 to 1980  was
carried out, was  presented  at the 1987  Lead-Blood Pressure Symposium  by Cooper
(1988).  Mortality causes were  coded  according to  the  seventh  (1955)  revision
of the  International  Classification of Diseases.   Data  were from deaths of
4,519 battery plant workers and  2,300  lead production or  smelter workers  during
this time period.  Cooper reported  that there were significant  excess deaths
for "other  hypertensive disease"  (444-447)  and "chronic  nphritis"  (592-594);
but deaths  from other  hypertension-related diseases did  not show  comparable
excesses and  renal  cancer  deaths  were fewer than  expected.   Also,  Selevan
et al.   (1988)  reported at the 1987 Symposium  that  an  analysis of causes of
mortality in 1,281 lead smelter workers employed at an Idaho smelter between
1940 and 1965 did not suggest an association between occupational lead exposure
and mortality  from hypertension.  On  the other hand,  the data  do  suggest an
association between lead and renal  disease and, possibly,  renal  cancer.
     The 1988  Symposium report  by  Pocock  et al.  (1988)  not only  reviewed
relationships between blood lead concentration and  blood  pressure as determined
in the  British  Regional Heart Survey,  but also looked  at  relationships between
lead and more  serious  cardiovascular  outcomes.   The results of  these further
analyses of the  BRHS  data set were reported by  Pocock et al.  (1988)  to  show
that,  by 6  years of  follow-up,  316  of  the men  had major  ischemic heart disease
and 66  had  a  stroke.   After  controlling for the confounding effects  of  ciga-
rette smoking  and town of residence,  statistical analyses  did  not  yield  sta-
tistically  significant  associations between blood  lead levels  and  such cardio-
vascular events.  However,  as the blood lead-blood pressure association  is so
weak,  Pocock et al.  (1988)  noted,  it is unlikely that any consequent associa-
tion between lead and  cardiovascular  disease could be demonstrated from pros-
pective epidemiological studies.
     On the other hand, Schwartz (1989) has recently reported results derived
from  new  NHANES II data  analyses,  which provide  evidence   for  significant

3/27/89                              18

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
associations between blood  lead  levels  and electrocardiogram (ECG)  abnormal-
ities indicative of left ventricular hypertrophy (LVH).   Such ECG abnormalities
represent an early indicator of cardiovascular disease that is much more common
than frank  myocardial  infarctions.   The logistic regression analyses employed
by Schwartz  (1989) yielded  a small (0.028) but  statistically significant  (p
<0.01) coefficient for  an  association between blood lead levels and increased
prevalence  of  LVH, taking  into account age,  race  and  sex (all  of which were
significantly  related to LVH at  p< 0.01,  <0.0001,  and <0.0001,  respectively).
The  interaction  terms  for  sex  or race  differences,  however,  were non-
significant (p> 0.20) with  regard to the cardiovascular effects of lead.
     The  Schwartz  (1989)  results  are consistent with  previous reports of
cardiovascular effects  being associated  with high levels of lead  exposure,
e.g., Kirkby  and Gyntelberg's 1985  report of  a 20% increase  (p <0.01) in
ischemic  changes as  coded  on the Minnesota Codes  in  lead workers  as compared
to controls matched  on  age,  race, and  several  other  pertinent  factors.  The
new  findings by  Schwartz,  however, point toward small  but not inconsequential
increased risks  for  serious  cardiovascular outcomes being associated with  the
relatively  low range of blood  lead  levels encountered  in  the  general  U.S.
adult population.   It  remains to  be  determined  to what extent  the  observed
lead effects on  left ventricular  hypertrophy  or  other cardiovascular functions
are  due  to  a  lead-induced  increase in  blood  pressure  or to some other lead-
related pathogenic mechanism.
     At the 1988 Symposium,  there was extensive discussion concerning contro-
versy in  the published  epidemiology  literature  about the nature of  a relation-
ship between blood lead and blood pressure.  As summarized by Tyroler (1988),
the  general population  studies discussed  above were seen as pointing toward a
causal  relationship  between  increases in  blood-lead levels and  significant and
increases in blood pressure,  extending to below values  below those currently
considered  to  be clinically  significant  as  hypertensive.   Tyroler further
noted that the association  has not been found in all studies and, when present,
has  been  such  that the increase  in blood pressure  with increase in blood lead
levels has  been  of small  absolute magnitude and not constant across age, sex,
and  race  subgroups.  Nevertheless,  despite the seemingly  small  elevations in
blood pressure when  viewed  from  the  clinical perspective  of each  individual,
Tyroler further  noted  that  the potential  public health importance of a blood-
lead blood-pressure  relationship  is  considerable due to  the strong association

3/27/89                              19

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
of blood  pressure with cardiovascular  morbidity  and mortality, the leading
cause(s) of death in our society,  and the large number of individuals exposed.
     Importantly  for  substantiating the  plausibility of blood-lead blood-
pressusre  associations  observed  in human populations, the  physiological  and
pharmacological regulation of blood  pressure  has  been studied  extensively by
experimental investigators.  Much of the  then available information on the
subject was discussed  in  the 1986  Addendum  (U.S.  EPA  1986b).  It is now under-
stood that various  hormonal  regulatory  systems, vascular smooth muscle,  and
heart contractility all contribute to the development of  blood pressure changes
Several  of  the  speakers at the  1988 Symposium  (Victery, 1988a) also reviewed
findings of changes in the cardiovascular system in  animals  exposed to lead.
For example, Victery  (1988b) reviewed the experimental studies  that have  been
conducted  over 40 years on the effects  of  lead on blood  pressure.   Differences
in animals  species, age at beginning of exposure, level  of  lead exposure,  and
the effects of  lead on blood pressure were described.   It  was  noted that in
several  of  the  high-dose  experiments, hypertension  was  observed, but  nephro-
toxicity of lead  may  have contributed to its  development.   In one  experiment,
high lead  exposure may have  reduced an  elevated blood pressure.  In contrast,
lower dose  experiments  consistently  demonstrated  a  hypertensive effect.  The
data suggest  that a  biphasic dose-response relationship may exist.   Future
research should be able to characterize  in  animals dose-response relationships
for blood  pressure effects across  low-level lead  exposure ranges most  applica-
ble to the general human population.
     With  regard  to  mechanisms  potentially underlying  lead-blood  pressure
relationships, Vander (1988)  reviewed the chronic effects of lead on the renin-
angiotensin system (one of the  primary regulatory hormones for blood pressure).
The changes observed  in both animals and humans  are  highly variable and  are
deoendent  on the  lead exposure  level,  the time of exposure, and other  stimuli
of the  renin-angiotensin   system.   The  human  data  are  consistent  with the
tentative  hypothesis  that lead-exposed  persons may have higher plasma renin
activity  (PRA)  than normal during periods  of  modest exposure  but  normal or
depressed  PRA following more chronic severe exposures.
     In another 1988 Symposium paper, the effects of lead on vascular reactivity
were described by Chai and Webb (1988).   There is convincing evidence that lead
alters vascular reactivity in lead-exposed animals,   which demonstrated a 15 to
20 mm Hg increase in systolic blood pressure.   Increased pressor responsiveness
to catecholamines has  been demonstrated in an  enhanced contraction  of  isolated
3/27/89                              20

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


vascular smooth muscle  to  adrenergic  agonists.   Further experimental  evidence
suggests that  there are alterations  in  the mechanisms that regulate  intra-
cellular calcium concentration  and  that  this may contribute to  the  abnormal
vascular function  in  lead-induced hypertension.  There were  further reports of
morphological, biochemical, and functional alterations in cardiovascular tissue
in the  reports  by  Kopp  et al. (1988) and  Boscolo  and Carmignani  (1988).  The
paper by Kopp  et al.  (1988)  in  the  Symposium volume  reviews  the  cardiovascular
actions of  lead,   including  evidence  that  lead  exposure (at least at  high
levels) leads to morphological,  biochemical, and functional derangements of the
heart.  The  experimental  literature also confirmed findings of cardiovascular
complications in experimental animals.   Findings include myocarditis,  electro-
cardiographic disturbances,  heightened catecholamine arrhymogenicity,  altered
myocardial  contractile  responsiveness to  inotropic  stimulation, dengerative
structural  and  biochemical changes  affecting the musculature of the heart and
vasculature,  hypertension, hyperchloesterolemia, atherosclerosis, and increased
vascular reactivity to  alpha-adrengergic agonists.   The precise nature of the
exposure-response  relationships that  apply are  still poorly characterized,  as
well  as the  exact  pathogenic mechanisms  for the  effects  of lead  on the cardio-
vascular system;   but importantly,  the  experimental  results provide  clear
evidence for lead causally affecting cardiovascular function.
     In the  1987 Symposium discussion session (Victery  et al.,  1988),  one of
the  invited  discussants,  Dr. Anthony Johns  proposed  a number  of critical
experiments  to  understand  how lead  may be affecting  blood pressure and whether
blood  pressure  changes  can  be  reversed  by removing lead from the diet.
Inhibitors  of the  angiotensin-converting enzyme (now used as antihypertensive
therapeutic  agents) could  be tried  to determine if this can prevent the blood
pressure increase  during  lead exposure.   The levels of  intracellular  calcium
should be measured in vascular  tissue and the pharmacologic agents that block
calcium or  potassium channels in cell  membranes  should be examined to determine
if they might reduce the effects of lead.

CONCLUSIONS
     With regard to the effects of  lead  on blood pressure, the  new  information
emerging since preparation of the 1986 Addendum, overall, substantiates further
the main conclusions  stated  in  that Addendum.   Sufficient evidence exists  from
both  the four large-scale general  population studies discussed above (NHANES II,

3/27/89                              21

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


BRHS, and the  two  Welsh  studies) and numerous other  smaller-scale studies to
conclude that a small  but positive association exists  between  blood  lead levels
and increases  in blood pressure.   Quantitatively,  the relationship  appears  to
hold across a  wide range of blood-lead values, extending possibly down to as
low as 7 ug/dl  for middle-aged men and,  furthermore, an estimated mean increase
of about 1.5-3.0 mm Hg in  systolic blood pressure appears to occur for every
doubling of blood  lead concentration  in adult males  and  something  less than
1.0-2.0 mm Hg  for  adult  females.  The  plausibility of these  relationships
observed in epidemiologic studies of human populations being of a causal  nature
is supported by controlled  experimental  animal studies  demonstrating  increased
blood pressure effects clearly attributable  to lead,  with an apparent biphasic
dose-response  relationship  being  involved  (i.e.  blood pressure elevations at
low lead dose  levels  and possible blood pressure reductions at very high lead
exposure levels).
     The implications of  lead-induced  blood-pressure  increases with regard  to
potential increased risk  for  other, more  serious cardiovascular  outcomes  still
remain  to  be  more clearly delineated.   As noted by  Tyroler and other
discussants at the 1988  Symposium mentioned  above,  essentially any increase  in
blood pressure carries with it likely  increased risk  (albeit  however small)
for stroke, heart  attack,  and/or associated mortality.  As such, projections
of potential lead  effects  on  such outcomes, as were  modeled by  Pirkle et al.
(1985)  and  discussed  in  the 1986  Addendum,  are  not unreasonable in  view of
the potential  very large public  health impacts;  however,  much caution must  be
exercised  in  accepting  the validity  of any specific quantitative estimates
derived  from  such projections in  view  of the uncertainties associated with
selection of the  specific  coefficients  used for (1) blood-lead blood-pressure
relationships  and  (2) relationships  between blood pressure increases and more
serious  cardiovascular outcomes.   The  difficulty  in  directly  demonstrating
associations between  lead  exposure and  stroke, heart  attacks,  etc.  lies  in  the
very large  study  cohorts (many thousands of subjects) that would be necessary
to have  sufficient statistical  power to detect the relatively small  increased
risk  levels  expected for  the more serious  cardiovascular  outcomes.    Some
newly available results  (i.e.  those  of  Schwartz,  1989)  from at least  one  large
scale study  help   to  illustrate  the  possibility  of detecting  indications of
such small increased risks  in the general population.
3/27/89                              22

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
III.  NEUROBEHAVIORAL AND GROWTH EFFECTS IN INFANTS AND CHILDREN

     A major advance in the epidemiological investigation of the health effects
of  lead  occurred with  the advent of  independent but somewhat  coordinated
prospective studies of  child  development.   Some of the  results  of four such
studies were discussed  in  the 1986 Addendum (U.S.  EPA,  1986b).   Much of the
same information, with  updating,  was  also presented in  a  critical  review  and
interpretation by Davis  and  Svendsgaard (1987).  The four prospective studies
in  question were  conducted in Boston,  Cincinnati, Cleveland,  and  Port  Pirie,
Australia.
     Based on  an assessment  of these studies, the  1986 Addendum  concluded
that fetal  lead exposure  could have  undesirable  effects  on  infant  mental
development,   length  of  gestation,  and  possibly  other  aspects of fetal
development,  with the  most consistent  evidence pertaining to  neurobehavioral
function.   In particular,  "All  of these studies taken together  suggest that
neurobehavioral  deficits, including declines in Bayley Mental Development Index
scores  and other  assessments  of neurobehavioral function,  are associated with
prenatal  blood lead exposure levels on the order of 10 to 15 pg/dl  and possibly
even lower, as  indexed  by  maternal  or cord blood  lead concentrations" (U.S.
EPA, 1986b, p.  A-48).
     This update  summarizes and assesses the  evidence from  these  and other,
more recent studies, both  individually  and collectively, and draws  conclusions
regarding their implications for regulatory decision-making.

Boston
     As noted in the 1986 Addendum,  a  series of reports by Bellinger,  Needleman,
and their  colleagues  (Bellinger et al. , 1984a, 1985,  1986a,b) described the
results of a  longitudinal  study of early  neurobehavioral  development through
the first  two  years of  life in  a cohort  of  Boston children.   More  recent
updates on this work have since  been  published by Bellinger  et al.  (1987a,
1989a), covering the same  period of development.  The latter reports confirm
that performance on the Bayley Mental  Development Index (MDI) at 6, 12, 18, and
24 months postnatally  is inversely  related to  cord blood lead levels  at birth
and that  the  amount of  deficit  in infants with  cord blood lead  levels of
10-25 (jg/dl is 0.25-0.5 standard deviations, or approximately 4-8 points on the
MDI (Table 2).   Moreover, Bellinger et al.  (1989a) found that this  association

3/27/89                              23

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE  OR CITE
              TABLE 2.   INFANTS'  MENTAL DEVELOPMENT INDEX SCORES
                      ACCORDING TO CORD BLOOD LEAD  GROUP
Cord Blood
Lead Group

Unadjusted score
Low
Medi urn
High
6 Months

109.
108.
106.

2 ±
6 ±
1 ±
Mean ±
12.9
12.0
11.1
Mental Development Index Score
12 Months 18 Months
24 Months
Standard Deviation
113.
115.
108.
1 ±
4 ±
7 ±
12.5
12.9
12.8
113.4
116.6
109.5
± 15.
± 16.
± 17.
5
7
5
115.9
119.9
110.6
± 17.2
± 14.4
± 16.5
                             Mean ± Standard Error
Controlled for
Low
Medi urn
High
p value**
No. of infants
potential confounders*
110.2 ±1.3 114.7 ±1.6
108.0 ±1.3 114.4 ±1.5
105.9 ±1.4 108.9 ±1.6
0.095
201
0.020
199
116.2 ±1.9
114.8 ±1.9
109.5 ±2.0
0.049
187
118.9 ±1.8
117.8 ± 1.7
111.1 ±1.8
0.006
182
 *Least-squares mean ± standard error,  derived from regression equations that
  included 12 potential  confounders and cord blood lead group coded as two
  indicator variables.
**Indicates p value associated with the F ratio that evaluates whether the
  mean Mental Development Index for any cord blood lead group differed
  significantly from the common mean after potential confounders were
  controlled for.

Source:   Bellinger et al.  (1987a).

was evident  in  several  different regression models and was not an artifact of
the approach they used  in  analyzing the data.   In  addition,  more detailed
analyses showed that  the  average MDI deficit in high cord lead infants (blood
lead  levels  of 10-25 ng/dl) was not due to a disproportionate  influence  of
results from infants  at higher (e.g.,  >15 (JQ/dl) blood lead levels.  That is,
the MDI  effect was  evident across  the  entire range  of  blood lead levels
starting at  10 ug/dl, which reinforces the previous selection of 10-15 ug/dl as
a  blood  lead  level  of  concern for early developmental  deficits (U.S.  EPA,
1986b).
     Other  recent analyses by  Bellinger et al.  (1988)  have  focused on the
interaction  of lead-related Bayley  MDI  deficits  with socioeconomic status
(SES).   Even  in  the relatively  advantaged  cohort of the  Boston study,
3/27/89                              24

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
 infants from other than the highest SES grouping tended to have lower covariate-
 adjusted scores on the MDI.  Moreover, the second-year MDI performance of these
 "lower" SES children was adversely affected at lower blood lead levels than was
 the performance of the higher SES children.  Specifically, within the lower SES
 grouping,  infants with  mid-range cord blood lead  levels  of  6-7 ug/dl scored
 significantly worse  on  the Bayley MDI than  infants  with  low cord blood lead
 levels of <3 |jg/dl.
     Bellinger et al.  (1987b,  1989b)  have also recently  reported  preliminary
 results of  later  testing  of 170 children  of  the Boston  cohort  at about  5 years
 (57 months)  of  age  on the McCarthy  Scales of Children's Abilities.   They found
 that  the  association between cord  blood  lead  and cognitive performance,  as
 measured by  the  General  Cognitive Index  (GCI) of  the McCarthy Scales,  was no
 longer  statistically significant at  57  months.    However,  the relationship
 between blood lead  level  at 24 months postnatally and  GCI scores  was statis-
 tically  significant, even  after adjusting  for covariates  and confounders
 (Table 3).   The mean 24-month blood lead level  was 6.8 ug/dl  (SD = 6.3).  Other
 postnatal blood lead measurements at 18 and 57  months were consistent with  this
 association but failed  to achieve statistical  significance.    The  size  of  the
 deficit amounted  to approximately  3  GCI  points for every natural  log  unit
 increment in blood lead level.
     Bellinger et al.  (1989b)  examined the change in cognitive performance of
 children from 24  to  57 months of age  in  relation to pre- and postnatal lead
 exposure levels and  various sociodemographic variables.  Improvement in rela-
 tive performance  was associated with  lower  blood lead levels  at  57 months,
 higher SES,  higher  HOME  scores,  higher maternal  IQ, and  female gender.  Con-
 versely, the  risk of  an  early  deficit persisting to  5  years of age  was
 increased  in  children with higher  prenatal  lead  exposure (10-25 ug/dl) and
 either high postnatal exposure or less favorable sociodemographic factors.   For
 example, "if two children with high cord blood  lead achieved the same MDI  score
 at 24 months, but one  had a low  blood  lead level  [<3 ug/dl]  at 57  months while
 the other  had a  high [>10 ug/dl]  level, the  child  with  lower exposure would be
 expected to  have  a  GCI  score that  is 0.61 standard normal  deviate units
 higher...,  [which]  corresponds  to  a  difference of  9.8 points" (Bellinger
et al.,  1989b).   Similar  comparisons of males  and  females with high  cord blood
 lead levels  indicated  that boys  scored 7.7 points  lower than girls on the  GCI,
and children in the  lower SES grouping scored  13.3 points lower than those in
the higher  SES grouping.
3/27/89                               25

-------









ac
t— t
O
LU
t—
»— 1
CJ
O
to

- Z
3:
i— 3;
a: 
•r-
4_>
ra

•r-
1 *
C.
ro

0-






1 0)
r- U
ra c
3 ra
-P E
a. s-
ai o
u s-
i. s.
01 01
a. a.






,_
03
JD
i.













rH
O













4_)
C i—
*J aj *o a> *~N
ro E O > in
flj O 01 JZ
Q) I- r- _J 4J
cn 3 ca c

s: -J
rH
00
CD
CO
•H cn

CM
O
CD



CO
CO
CD
CO
+ 1 co

CM
00
O





diD
f**-
.
0
cn
•H cn
,
o
0

0







r^
^x
.
O

4*1 co
.
CM

,
O
1




co
cn
CD
CM
+\ «*•
•
in

.
CD





«
cn
CM
. -K
rH*

+1 CO
,
oo
CM
.
O













LO




O
00
o
i-H
•H CO

CM
00
CD
1


rH
00
CD
H
•H cn

on
o
d
1




^«
f*x
,
0
0
-H on

^ft
O

C5







cn
r^
.
o
CM
•H »*•

^>
u>

^3
1




cn
00
CD

•H CM
•
00
cn

CD
1





in
CM

r-i
in
+•1 CM
•
co
^
.
rH
1












CM
rH



cn
co
o
*4*
•H r^.

o
CO
CD



rH
cn
CD
f*^.
•H in
,
CM
in
CD
i




CM
CO

0
in
•H i-H

CD
CM

rH
1






(^
00

o
rH
4-1 «!•

CM

.
O
1




rH
O
r-i
in
•H CO
•
cn
rH
.
O
1





en
CO
.
rH
m
+1 CM
,
CM
to
.
rH













00
rH



CM
cn
o
co
-H co

o
cn
0
1


*«^
CTv
CD
cn
•H «*

WO
u>
C3
i




m
00
.


+) o

in

.
rH
1






00
00

o *i-
0
+t o

CO
in
.
CM
1




,-f
0
r-J
cn
4H U3

rH

.
0
1





CM

.
rH O
^-
•H 0

in
cn

CM
t












^j-
CM



in
rH
r-J
O
+1 rH

cn
00
r-i
1


in
CM
rH
O
+1 !-•
,
cn
«*
0





co
rH
.
rH
rH
•H cn
.
CO
i-l
,
O







co
rH

r-J CM

•H 0

CO
CO

CM
1




00
co
r-i

•H *r

LO
0

t— t
1





00
00

r-i
ro
•H CM
,
00
CM

CM













r*^
m







































































.
i.
o
j_
i.
01
•a
i.
ra
T3
c
m
-U
in

+1

a>
4-)
ro
e

4_>
in
0)

i_
at
4->
01
E
(0

ro
a.
•K




























,
O
t-
a>
N

in
•r-

0)
4J
ra
E
•r-
^J
in
0)

i.
01
^>
01
E
ro
i_
(O
a.

at
+J

4-1
ro
5
in
•r-
in
01
J^
4->
o
a.
>J
-C

r—
•—
c

a*
jz
+j

_f~
4J
*r~
i

-a
O)

ro
»(—
u
0
in
in
ro

ai
^
^—
ro

1
a.
•K
*








































































,
'~*
pS
00
cn
rH
**^

.
r—
ro

4^
Ol

i.
01
CT
c
•r-
r—

01
CO



oi
u
i.
31
o
OO

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  00 NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     Bellinger  et  al.  (1987b)  also  evaluated the  degree of  disparity  in
McCarthy  subscale  performance,  since learning disabilities  are  often highly
specific  to certain  cognitive  functions.   A greater  number of  subscale
discrepancies was  associated with higher (>10 ug/dl) concurrent  (57 month)
blood lead  levels.

Cincinnati
     Interim results of  a longitudinal  study of  inner-city  children  born in
Cincinnati, Ohio were  reported  by Dietrich et al.  (1986,  1987a)  and summarized
in the  1986 Addendum.   Structural equation modeling, a  statistical method of
examining  relationships   among  factors  that  may be  both independent and
dependent  variables  or mediators  of effects, indicated  that prenatal lead
exposure had an indirect effect on 6-month MDI and PDI (Psychomotor Development
Index)  scores  through  its  effects  on gestational age and/or birth  weight.
Higher  prenatal  (maternal) blood  lead  levels were  associated with  reduced
gestational age  and reduced birth weight, which in turn were  significantly
associated with reduced MDI and PDI performance.
     Although the Cincinnati subjects were not grouped by blood lead levels for
these analyses, separate  analyses of birth weight effects by Bornschein  et al.
(1989)  investigated the  dose-response  relationship by  grouping into five
6-|jg/dl  intervals.   An  increase in the  percentage of low birthweight newborns
between the 7-12 (jg/dl  grouping and  the S13  pg/dl grouping implied the possi-
bility of  a threshold  for low birthweight effects  (categorically  defined as
less than  2750  g)  in  the vicinity of 12-13 ug/dl,  although  a precise deter-
mination is not possible  and could extend as  low as  7 ug/dl  or perhaps as  high
as 18 ug/dl.  To the extent that low birthweight mediated the effect of lead on
Bayley scores in the  Cincinnati study,  the inferred information is consistent
with the conclusion (U.S.  EPA,  1986b) that a  blood lead level of 10-15 ug/dl, and
possibly lower, constitutes a level  of  concern for  impaired  performance  on the
Bayley MDI.
     Dietrich et al. (1987b, 1989a,b) later reported more complete results for
neurobehavioral  developmental outcomes  in approximately  300  infants  from  the
Cincinnati  study (Table 4).  The  children were tested on the Bayley Scales at
3, 6, 12,  and  24  months  of age.  After adjusting for covariates,  deficits in
3- and 6-month  Bayley  MDI scores were  significantly associated  with  prenatal
(maternal) and cord blood lead  levels, confirming the earlier, preliminary

3/27/89                              27

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO  NOT QUOTE  OR  CITE
          TABLE 4.   RESULTS OF  MULTIPLE REGRESSION ANALYSES EXAMINING
                   THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BLOOD LEAD  LEVEL
       AND PERFORMANCE ON THE BAYLEY MENTAL INDEX AT 3-24 MONTHS OF  AGE*
Blood
Lead
Measure (|jg/dl)

Prenatal
Umbilical cord
10 day
3 month

Prenatal
Prenatal by
Child Sex
10 day
10 day by SES

Prenatal
Umbilical cord
10 day

Prenatal
10 day
3 month
Maximum 1st Year
Maximum 2nd Year
24 months
N

228
80
261
n. r.

249

283


258
98
257

237
270
270
270
270
270
Standard
Beta Error
3-month MDI
-.34
-.60
.06
-.23
6-month MDI
-0.89
1.53
-3.15
0.16
12-month MDI
0.09
-0.17
-0.62
24-month MDI
0.51
-0.02
0.24
0.24
0.10
0.13

0.17
0.26
0.22
0.18

0.34
0.51
1.30
0.08

0.26
0.36
0.31

0.22
0.25
0.21
0.10
0.07
0.09
t
Value

-1.96
-2.30
0.26
-1.30

-2.60
2.98
-2.43
2.13

n. r.
n. r.
n. r.

2.31
-0.07
1.12
1.39
1.39
1.45
P
Value**

.05
.02
.79
.20

0.009
0.003
0.016
0.034

N.S.
N.S.
<0.04

0.022
0.948
0.262
0.166
0.166
0.149
 *Betas adjusted for different sets of covariates for each age of MDI testing.
**Two-tailed values.
Sources:   Dietrich et al.  (1987b, 1988, 1989b).
3/27/89                              28

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
analyses of  Dietrich et al.  (1987a).   The magnitude of the  deficit  in the
3-month MDI  amounted  to  6  points for every  10-pg/dl  increment in cord blood
lead;  the  corresponding  6-month  MDI  deficit was nearly 7  points.   Also,  a
conservative reanalysis  (Dietrich et  al.,  1989b)  of  the  6-month  data  using  all
potential  covariates  and confounders  confirmed earlier findings,  although
interactive  effects with gender and SES were evident  (see  below).   However,
neither prenatal nor  cord  blood lead measures were  significantly related  to
12-month MDI scores,  although the association between neonatal (10-day) blood
lead levels  and  12-month (as well as 6-month)  MDI  scores  remained statisti-
cally  significant  (Dietrich  et al. ,  1989a).   It should be  noted that analyses
of the 12-month data are not yet final.
     By  24  months, no statistically  significant  negative  relationships were
detectable between  lead  exposure variables (either pre- or postnatal) and MDI
scores (Dietrich et al. ,  1989b).  Only one  parameter  estimate (for prenatal
blood  lead) achieved statistical significance (p = 0.0217),  and it was positive
rather than negative.
     In addition,  the  Bayley Infant Behavior Record (IBR)  was administered at
12 and 24 months  to  assess  the infants'  social and  emotional  development
(Dietrich et al.,  1989a,b).   Factor  analysis of the 30 IBR items at 24 months
yielded principal  factors  of Sustained  Attention, Activity  Level,  and Positive
Mood,  which  were used in  regression  analyses to assess the  effect of lead
exposure.  Lower scores  on  Sustained Attention and Positive Mood factors were
both significantly  associated with  increased neonatal  (10-day)  blood lead
levels, consistent  with  the 12-month MDI relationship to neonatal  blood lead
(Dietrich et al. ,  1989a).   However,  none of the 24-month IBR results achieved
statistical significance.  Bayley PDI results were incomplete.
     Dietrich et al.  (1987b,  1989b)  also found that gender and SES interacted
with the lead  exposure-MDI  relationship.   Male infants  and  children  from  the
lower  half of  the  SES distribution for this cohort were more sensitive to the
effects of early lead exposure on neurobehavioral development.   For  example,
male infants showed an 8.67-point deficit on the 6-month MDI for every 10-ug/dl
increment in prenatal blood lead, and infants below the sample median SES score
had a  7.57-point deficit for every 10-ug/dl  increment  in neonatal  blood  lead
(Dietrich et al. , 1989b).
     Dietrich et al.  (1989b)  interpreted their failure to  detect a persistent
effect of  fetal  lead  exposure on the 24-month  Bayley  Scales as probably due

3/27/89                              29

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


to a  neurobehavioral  catch-up response,  similar  to that observed in  infant
twins (Wilson, 1986)  or  other infants compromised during prenatal  development
(Tanner, 1981).  Exploratory  analyses  indicated  that the greatest percentage
increase in  MDI  raw  scores  (number of  items  passed)  from age 3 months to
24 months was  inversely  related to  prenatal  lead  exposure,  birth weight,
gestation,  and  head circumference  (Dietrich et  al. ,  1989b).   Thus,   those
infants with  the highest prenatal  blood lead levels, lowest  birth weight,
shortest gestation, or smallest  head  circumference showed the  greatest degree
of catch-up in postnatal  neurobehavioral  development.
     As previously  noted,  the Cincinnati  study  has also  produced  evidence
of direct effects  of  prenatal lead exposure on infant physical development at
birth.  Bornschein et al. (1989)  reported that initial  results  from 202 infants
showed  an  inverse  relationship between  maternal  blood lead levels and both
birth weight  and length.  Maternal blood  lead samples were obtained  at the
first prenatal visit (between 6 and 28 weeks of gestation; mean = 16 wks).   The
mean maternal blood  lead level  was 7.6  ug/dl  (range:  1-26  |jg/dl).  Exclusion
criteria included birth weight less than 1500 g,  less than 35 weeks  gestational
age at  birth,  twin birth, and serious  medical  conditions.   Regression  analyses
considered 21 potential confounders and covariates.  The final  regression  model
for birth weight  showed  that  log maternal  blood  lead was  significantly related
to birth weight,  but interacted with  maternal age to produce  a significant
(p <0.007)  reduction  in  covariate-adjusted birth weight.   Thus,  the effect of
each natural  log increment in blood lead varied from a birthweight reduction  of
58.1 g  for  18-year-old mothers  to  a  reduction  of 600.1 g for  30-year-old
mothers.  Maternal  blood lead and  race  interacted to produce  a significant
reduction in  birth  length in  white  infants (p  <0.025).  Thus,  the birth length
of white infants  decreased  ~2.5  cm for each natural log increment in  maternal
blood  lead.   Maternal  blood  lead  showed  no  significant  relationship to
covariate-adjusted  head  circumference  or  gestation  length.    Obstetrical
complications and  Apgar  scores  also showed  no relationship with  lower birth
weight or lead exposure.
     Separate analyses of 861 women (including the 202 subjects just described)
from the Cincinnati cohort,  but including pregnancies of not less than 20  weeks
duration, also showed  a  highly significant  (p <0.0006)  negative relationship
between maternal blood  lead level  and covariate-adjusted  birth  weight (Born-
schein  et al.,  1989).   Overall,  this  cohort had  a decrease of approximately
114 g in birth weight for each natural log increment in maternal blood lead.
3/27/89                              30

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     Analyses of  postnatal  growth  rates in a cohort  of  260  children  from  the
Cincinnati study suggested an interactive effect of prenatal  and postnatal  lead
exposure  (Shukla  et  al. ,  1987).   Splitting prenatal blood lead levels and the
average  increase  in  postnatal  blood lead levels at the  median  (7.7 ug/dl  for
prenatal, 3.4 ug/dl  for the postnatal increase) provided a matrix of low/low,
low/high, high/low,  and  high/high  exposure conditions.   Analysis of  data  for
129  subjects  in the  high prenatal exposure  classification  indicated  that
covariate-adjusted growth rates  for  stature  over 3-15  months  of age  were
significantly (p = 0.006) and negatively related to the postnatal increase in
blood  lead  level.   No effect was  seen  for  the  low prenatal  exposure group.
Thus, for infants whose prenatal  blood lead levels were greater than 7.7 ug/dl,
there would be,  on  average, a 2-cm difference  in  height at  15 months of age
between  those infants who experienced no increase in  postnatal  blood lead and
those who experienced an increase of ~10 ug/dl.
     Other recent  results of  a  neurobehavioral nature  from the Cincinnati
study have also  been reported by Bhattacharya  et  al.  (1988,  1989).   Postural
sway was  assessed by an automated apparatus  in  33 children  at  6  years  of  age
(Bhattacharya et al., 1988).  Initial  results indicated that  maximum blood lead
level during the  second year of life was  significantly  related to degree of
postural sway or imbalance at 6 years.  Blood lead levels in  this sample peaked
during the second year,  averaging  25.6  ug/dl  (range:  9.3-49.4 ug/dl), measured
quarterly.  The  later  report by  Bhattacharya et al.  (1989) covers 63 children
and  confirms the earlier results showing a  significant  relationship  between
sway and  second  year maximum blood lead (r = 0.34, p =  0.02).   Although more
test results are needed  to fully explore these effects,  these preliminary
findings  provide  additional  indications of neurobehavioral  disruption  during
early childhood exposure to lead.

Cleveland
     As  noted in  the 1986 Addendum (U.S. EPA,  1986b),  the prospective  study
conducted by Ernhart  and  her colleagues has  provided  some direct as well  as
indirect evidence of  an  effect of prenatal lead exposure  on neurobehavioral
development.   Ernhart et  al.  (1985a,  1986) reported  significant  associations
between  cord blood  lead  levels  and measures of Abnormal  Reflexes (on the
Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral  Assessment Scale: NBAS) and Neurological Soft
Signs (on  the  Graham-Rosenblith  Behavioral Examination  for  Newborns:   G-R).

3/27/89                              31

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


Also, the G-R Muscle Tonus measure was significantly  related to maternal blood
lead levels at  delivery.   However,  restricting the analyses to only 132 pairs
of mother-infant  data,  Ernhart et al. found  only  the G-R Neurological  Soft
Signs to be significantly  related to  cord blood  lead.  A brief report  on later
outcomes in this  same  cohort  mentioned  a  significant association  between
performance on  the G-R  Neurological  Soft Signs  scale  and  12-month  MDI  scores
(Wolf et al.,  1985).   Thus,  it is possible  to infer a relationship between
cord blood lead levels  and 12-month MDI  performance  in the Cincinnati  study,
although Ernhart et al.  (1985a, 1986) did not conclude that such an association
exists.   Since the mean cord blood lead was  5.89 ug/dl and the  maximum was  only
14.7 ug/dl, any effect  of  prenatal  lead  exposure necessarily occurred  at blood
lead levels below 15 ug/dl.
     Later reports by  Ernhart  and her colleagues (Ernhart  et al.,  1987a, 1988;
Ernhart and Morrow-Tlucak,  1989)  presented  more complete  results  from their
continuing longitudinal study (Table 5).   The Bayley MDI  was administered at 6,
12, and 24 months;  the Bayley PDI and the Kent Infant Development Scale (KID)
at 6 months;  and the Stanford-Binet IQ test  at 36 months.   Postnatal blood lead
levels were measured at 6,  24,  and 36 months.   A total of 285 children from the
original cohort of 389  were  sampled for  blood lead levels; N's for individual
analyses ranged from 109 to  165.  After  control  for covariates, maternal blood
lead accounted  for a  significant  amount  of the  variance  in 6-month MDI, PDI,
and KID scores.   Although  these three relationships were all  negative (higher
PbB associated  with  lower developmental  scores),  concurrent blood  lead was
positively related to the 6-month  KID and accounted for nearly as much variance
as did  maternal blood  lead.   Maternal blood  lead  levels  averaged  6.50 ug/dl,
with a  maximum  of 11.8 |jg/dl; 6-month blood lead levels  averaged 10.05 ug/dl,
with a maximum of 24.00 ug/dl.
     More recent  results from  testing these  children at  age 4  years,  10 months
on the  Wechsler  Preschool  and Primary  Scale  of Intelligence (WPPSI)  were
reported by Ernhart  et al. (1987b).  Analyses were based on N's  ranging from
117 to 211.  Although bivariate correlations were statistically significant for
all  PbB-WPPSI  relationships  except  6-month  PbB,  in no case did PbB account for
a  significant  amount  of the variance (by two-tailed t test) after control  for
13 covariates (Table 6).
3/27/89                              32

-------


JTCOMES
o
r-
a.
0
LU
LU
CC
LU

C 0
E -P
I- JZ O

ca re o>
J3 -f O
Q. J- C
re re
0 Z-

(U fl>
^> o
•f- 10
i. •!-
re i-
> re
0
s_

z

/el opmental
Measure
O)
a
OJ
•o t-
O 73 3
o re in
•— ai re
oa — i a>

tn I-H in us o to r~» r-- r-- K»- rH CM
rHi-HrHOOO OrHOrHOrH
1 1 1 1 1 1 1 -f 4- 1 1 1
rHrOCMVDOO IDCnrHCOCMlD
iDcnoocsiocM in^-in»i-ooo
rHOrHCSOO OCMOrHOO
000000 000000
oooooo oooooo
*
*
* * *
CMCSICMVOCOCD VO«J3f^-l — rH"*-
rHrHCMi-H«*-O t~-CMO»}-CMin
CMCMCOOOrH OOrHi-IOrH
cMincncMcocn loior^iDcom
ocnr-ocMin ^J-OOTJ-OCM
CO CM f^" O O O ^ O rH rH O rH
OOOOOO OOOOOO
OOOOOO OOOOOO

oin'*ri~«*''^' «*• i— i oo r~- us co
iDOOiDOOtDen ononrHCM^cn
^•^•^•COiDOO coco^rof^.ro
CMCMrHi-HCOCO CMrHrHCMCO^T
oooooo oooooo
* * * * *
*a-rHOO*t-«*-O ^-rHO«*mrH
1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
f^r»-oiLOCMOO rorocnr~mo
CMCMrH^J-^J-rO rHrHOCMCMCM

I—* f— I
1— 1 I-H O 1— 1 1— t O
CO CO I—* H>H I-H CO O. O. !•— * HH I-H CO
S. 0- *t 0 Q 1 S: Q- ^ Q Q 1
J= J= J= J= J= J=
•p4->4->S-i-(- +J +J +-) i- S- S-
cccrarere cccrerere
s: s: s: >->->- s: s: z >->->-
IDlOLOrHCMCO IDlDlDrHCMCO
re
c
i-
0) T3
•P S-
re o
z: u


PAL OUTCOMES
LU
CL.
3
LU
CD
ce

h-
i— i
oo
LU
_J
ca
CL.
K
1—
oo
o
a.
1—
LU
Of
in o
a> z
•P 
o
c
o
in
0)
s-
a

a. o
o



c
o
r— +J
a. re
OO J-
£-
O


opmental
01
OJ
a
T3
O T5
o re
r- 0)
ca -J
a.

Interaction
CX
o
+j
o
CJ

in
O)
re
••—
S-
o
o
K
CL

(.

Z
iasure
Measure Me
•«*• CO rH ro r-. r-CM OO
CO ^" tf") tiO CO ^D CO ^3

rHOOOO i-IO rH
OOOOO OO O
i-HcMinmcn incn oo
ro ro CM 01 «r cncM en

OOOOO OO O
o o o o o o o
OOOOO OO O
cocn«tocn CMrH CD
i-Hi-HrHCOCM COfO CO

O O O
cncno«*-in oo o
CM ^t" —tt~^
t—t H-«
o a I-H H- i ca t— i ca ca
s: a- o o i o i i
3Z ^  s: to oo
IDlDrHCMCO CMCO CO
•p t- i.
c re re
O O) O)
E >, >,
ID CM CO













*-^
CO
en
rH
Q
-P OO
in en
0> i-H
*re r-^
u re
Two-tailed statist
Source: Ernhart et
33

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
      TABLE 6.   THE RELATIONSHIP OF PRENATAL AND POSTNATAL LOG BLOOD LEAD
                     WITH IQ AT AGE FOUR YEARS,  TEN MONTHS
Blood
Lead
Measure
Prenatal
Maternal
Cord
Postnatal
6 Months
2 Years
3 Years
Mean
N

134
117

121
149
154
211
Full Scale
rB t

-.23**
-.21*

-.06
-.38**
-.32**
-.26**

-0.27
-0.54

-0.08
-0.69
+0.30
+0.48
iQa
Verbal Scale
r t

-.25**
-.22*

-.07
-.36**
-.39**
-.30**

-0.49
-0.78

-0.26
-1.41
-1.58
-1.00
Performance Scale
r t

-.23**
-.20*

-.08
-.34**
-.30**
-.26**

-0.48
-0.78

+0.10
+0.22
+0.96
+0.56
aWPPSI scores except for two S-B IQ scores;  latter two cases were necessarily
 excluded from the tests of the Verbal  and Performance Scales.
 r describes the unadjusted relationship of the PbB and IQ measures;  t is
 the test of the increment in variance  associated with the addition of PbB to
 the covariate variance in the IQ model.
*p <.05, **p <.01, two-tailed tests.
Source:   Ernhart et al. (1987b).

     Language development  in  the  Cleveland  cohort was also assessed at age 1,
2, and  3 years  (Morrow-Tlucak and Ernhart,  1987).   The Sequenced Inventory of
Communication Development  (SICD)  was  administered  to assess expressive  and
receptive language  development.   In addition,  productive  speech  (quantity,
length of utterance, vocabulary, communicative intent, and intelligibility) was
assessed at about 2 years.   The number  of subjects for each of the analyses was
not reported, but  the  number of blood lead measurements  over the  period from
delivery to  3 years ranged from 146 to  169.   Alpha was set at 0.01.   Several
bivariate correlations  achieved  statistical  significance, but no relationship
achieved significance in multivariate F tests, although the regression analysis
for cord PbB and mean  length of utterance was significant at p = 0.03.   By
backward elimination,  lead (cord  PbB)  remained in the model  (at p ^0.05) for
two measures  of  language  development:  mean  length  of utterance  and  one  aspect
of communicative intent ("expanded repetitions").   However, concurrent (2-year)
blood lead was  positively  related  to one aspect of  intelligibility (a decrease
in "unintelligible utterances").
3/27/89                              34

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     Preliminary analyses of  physical  growth in the preschool period  (up to
4 years,  10  months) revealed  no  significant negative  relationship between
either cord  blood  lead  or integrated postnatal  blood lead  and  height or weight
in  an  unspecified  number of  children  from the Cleveland  study  (Marler  and
Ernhart,  1987).   Earlier analyses  (Ernhart  et al. , 1986)  of  birth weight,
length,  and  head circumference had shown no  significant  effect of either
maternal blood lead (N = 185) or cord blood lead (N  = 162).

Port Pirie
     Vimpani et al.  (1985)  reported preliminary neurobehavioral results for the
prospective study conducted in Port Pirie, South Australia.  The Bayley MDI and
PDI were administered to 592 children at  age  24 months.   Initial results
indicated that a  decline in 24-month MDI  performance was significantly associ-
ated with  postnatal blood lead  levels at 6  months  or integrated  over the
24 months after birth.   Although  several  covariates were taken into account in
these partial  linear  regression analyses, maternal  IQ  had  been  measured  for
only part  of the  cohort and  HOME  scores had not yet  been included in the
analyses.  Nevertheless, as noted in the  1986 Addendum, the available  informa-
tion suggested that results from  the Port Pirie study  were consistent  with the
other prospective  studies  in  pointing to deficits on the Bayley MDI as  a
function of perinatal lead  exposure.
     Although postnatal   rather than prenatal  lead exposure appeared to play a
greater  role  in  the MDI deficits  of the  Port Pirie cohort, it has  been noted
that blood  lead  levels  increased  considerably after birth,  particularly from 6
to 15 months  of  age,  and that earlier  testing  on the  Bayley Scales (e.g.,  at
6 months) might have revealed a more significant relationship  with a measure of
prenatal lead exposure (U.S.  EPA,  1986b; Davis and Svendsgaard, 1987).   Indeed,
the Port  Pirie investigators  have since stated  that "the likely  greater impact
of the much higher levels of  PbB encountered postnatally"  may  have accounted in
part for the  difference  in  their  results  versus the other  prospective studies
(Wigg et al., 1988).
     Later  reports  from  the  Port  Pirie study have  noted  that the  effect  of
postnatal blood lead  levels  on the 2-year MDI was attenuated  as more complete
control   for maternal  IQ  and  HOME  scores  was  incorporated  into the analysis,
although all regression  coefficients for postnatal  blood lead  measures remained
negative  (Vimpani  et  al.,  1989;  Wigg  et  al. ,  1988;  Baghurst   et  al. ,  1987).

3/27/89                              35

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE  OR  CITE


Elevations  in  6-month  blood lead continued  to  have the greatest measurable
impact on 2-year MDI scores,  although the regression coefficient for 6-month
blood  lead  was only -0.16,  with p = 0.07,  after controlling for at least
15 covariates.   The  results  indicated  that "with  other  factors remaining
constant, a child's MDI  at  24 months will be 1.6 points (equivalent to 1.5%)
lower  for every 10  ug/dl  rise in PbB at  6  months of age	   Moreover,  in
view  of  the fit of  a  linear model  in  multiple regression analysis, these
findings provide no evidence of a threshold  effect"  (Wigg  et al., 1988).
     Continuing study of  the  Port Pirie cohort  has  yielded  results  for  neuro-
behavioral  development  at  age  four years  (McMichael  et al. , 1988).   The
McCarthy  Scales  of Children's Abilities  were administered  to  537  children
within 6  months of  their fourth birthday; N's  for  individual analyses ranged
from  463  to 534.   Multiple regression analyses incorporating 18 covariates
indicated that scores  on the  McCarthy GCI  were  significantly  related  to
log-transformed postnatal blood  lead  levels  at  6, 24, and 36  months as well  as
an integrated  average for the four-year postnatal  period (Table 7).  Similar
effects were also  evident for the McCarthy  Perceptual-Performance and Memory
Scales.
     The  largest  single  coefficient  (-15.0;  p  =  0.04)  was for  the  GCI  and
integrated postnatal average  blood  lead relationship, which  indicates that GCI
scores  decreased  by 15  points  for every 10-fold  increment in  blood lead.
Alternatively  stated, GCI scores declined approximately 7.2  points  as  blood
lead  levels increased from 10 to 30 pg/dl.  Further  analyses indicated that the
relationship between lead exposure  and GCI  was as  strong or  even stronger at
blood  lead levels below 25 ug/dl  than it was overall (geometric mean blood lead
level  peaked at  2  years:  21.2 ug/dl;  integrated postnatal average:   19 (jg/dl).
Other  analyses  revealed  no  indication that  GCI performance  at  four years  was
especially  influenced by  more recent blood  lead levels; rather, the effect of
lead  on the GCI appeared to  be cumulative across the entire postnatal period.
      Pregnancy outcomes  were also evaluated  in the Port Pirie cohort (McMichael
et al., 1986).  A  significantly  elevated risk of preterm (<37 weeks) delivery
was  associated with  maternal  blood lead levels above 14 ug/dl.   Neither birth
weight  nor  spontaneous abortions (<20 weeks) showed a  signficant association
with  blood  lead  levels.   However, blood lead levels of mothers who had still-
births  were significantly  lower  than those  who  had live  births  (7.9  vs.
10.4  ug/dl).   As discussed  in the 1986 Addendum and by Davis and Svendsgaard

3/27/89                               36

-------


to
LU
to

i
^
z


0
t— 1

to
LU

C3
LU
ce

LU

a.
I-H
^]
Z
Q
Z


LU *
—1 tO
Cu 0|
H-H UJ
to >•

^ 0£.
O =>

U_ LL.

Z LL.
O 0
»-H
H- LU
-
_i :c
03 r-
ce
o o
—1 U
z:
O LL.
0
to
r-

LU
1 — 1
CJ
t-H
U.
Ll_
LU
O
O

O
LU
h-

I-H
L_
to
LU


.
(•*»

LU
i
m

h-







're
>r.
4->

re
ai Q-
^—
re
u
to
;>,

O
E
at  Q.
a. E
0>-r-
u to
i.
01
Cu







r—
x re
OJ *r-
•73 4->
C S-
HH re
ai
>
1 i

c
cn
o
CJ

•— cu
ro r—
i. a.
0) E
C •!-

an
•H 00

•* 0

CD *~"






CM

CM <-x
CO
•H O

cn CD

^f>
1




f^
.
CM <"^
LO
•H on
CM CD
0







ro

CM *^N
CO
•HO

rH C>

in
t







ID
.
^ ****•
ro
•H cn

^ o
o
1





cn

CO 00
o
•H 0

«*• CD
• ^^
o
rH
1
















>,

ai
>
•r-
(—
01
a
ID
csi /•%

•H CM

to o
• s^
CM






o

csi ^^

•H rH
,
0 0

CO





^
.
CM /-N
o
•H LO
tO CD
rH







o

CM <->
co
•H rH

0 CD

CO
1







rH
.
^ ^^
CM
•H ^»-

CO CD
CO






^

CO/-S
00
•H 0
,
rH O
• *••*'
to
1





















•o
i.
o
CJ
in
csi ^
^-
+1 co

<• o
* v«^
CM
1





CM
.
CM /-\
CO
•H 0

(Tt O
• ^^
^
1




in
,
CM /~.
to
•H 0
oo o
^.
1






CM *~* -
• rH
CM O
O
-H .
0
00 V

p^
1







«*•

^ /"\
LO
•H 0

LO O
00
1





oo *~»
• rH
CO O
0
-H •
0
rH V

in
rH
1

















.C
4^
C
0
yr

to
^
csi »-s

+1 CO

ID O
• ^H^
O
1





CM
.
CM ^N
i-H
+1 0

CO CD
• ^^
LO
1




00
.
CM /-»
to
-H CM
CM CD
CO
1






CM <"»
• rH
CM O
O
-H •
O
CM V

00 ^^
1







00

^ ^^
to


LO CD
CO
1





00 ^^
• i— 1
ro o
0
•H .
O
ro v
• *«™^
^>
i-H
1
















j:
4->
C
o


ID
rH
rH
CO ^^
CM
+1 0

CO CD
• ***s
r-.
i





^ ^"N
. rH
CM CD
O
-H •
O
**• v
• SwX
CO
i-H
1



CM
•
ro <^^
ID
-H 0
CM CD
to
1






ID <•>
. rH
CM O
CD
-H •
O
ID V
* ^^^
CO
1— 1
1






^
.
tn ^^
*^
•H 0

i-H CD
i-H
rH
1




ro f***
. rH
««• o
O
•H •
0
00 V

in
CM
i
















JC
^_)
c
o


^^
CM
CM
CO **^
p^
•H CM

CM CD
• **-S
^,
1





^" ^**>
. rH

CM
•H 0
t^ d
fv.
i






in ^
• rH
CM O
O
•H •
O
to v
• ^^
ro
i— i
i






to

in /-»
CM
•H o

rH CD
CO
rH
1




co '-^
• rH
* O
0
•H •
O
rH V

LO
CM
1
















.C
^J
C
o
TL.

to
ro
o
CO *** s
pSB
+1 1-1

i-H 0
i '***S
*$•
1





ro '*•**
. rH
CM CD
O
•H •
O
CO V
• ****
o
rH
1



O
.
co *****
CO
-H rH
o d
^
i






^ ^^
• rH
CM O
O
•H •
O
CM V

rH
i— 1
1







rH
. /-^
LO cn
CM
•H •
o

in
i




rH s*~*
• rH
^f CD
O
-H .
O
rH V

d
CM
1
















£
^ *
C
o


00

CM
<3- s~^
to
•H 0

cn CD
. * — '
p*.
i





rH ****
• rH
CO CD
O
-H •
O
CO V
• ^^
CO
rH
1



rH

^* ^"N
ID
+1 O
co d
00 ^
i






CM <-v
• rH
CO O
O
-H •
CD
i-H V
• ^^
ID
rH
1






co
.
r- /-\
»*•
+1 0

o d
ID
rH
1




ID *~\
. i— 1
ID O
O
+1 •
O
cn v

00 "^
CM
1




, 	
re
+•*
re
c
4-1
U)
O
o.

T3
 ai
re cn
t- re

at at
4-> >
c re
I-H











j_
o
<4-

cu
(_
re

i/)
ai
3
,~-
re
>
a.
at
r-



CO
ai
4J
re
t-
re
>
o
u •
o
CO I-
I-H ai
M
01
c. +->
•r- O
4-> C
re
i- a*
O i.
o. re
t.
O tn
(J CU
C 4->
•r- re
E
10 •!-
ai 4->
in u>
>, 0)

re a>
tz ^z
re +->

o re
I- .C
1- 4->
ai m
t- T-
re to
O)
in -C
4-> 4->
c: o
ai Q.

o x:

H- -a
 ai
a> JT

O)
01 <*-
I- O

r— W
re 4->
•r- C
4-> O)
S- E
re m
a. u>

ai to
-C U)
i— re
*
                                                  §
                                                  cn
                                                   re

                                                  +>
                                                   ai
                                                   a>
                                                   re

                                                   o


                                                   u
                                                   a>
                                                   u
                                                   O
                                                  to
37

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


(1987),  this  seemingly  paradoxical  relationship  might reflect  increased
transfer of  lead from the mother  to  the fetus, resulting  in  greater  fetal
toxicity and stillbirth.   Vimpani  et al.  (1989) reported preliminary results  of
an analysis of  tissue  lead concentrations in cord  and  placental membrane  and
body that are consistent with this hypothesis.  Placental  membrane  and body
concentrations of lead were  higher in cases of  late  fetal  deaths and  preterm
births  than  for normal  births.   However,  the number of cases  analyzed was
limited  (e.g.,  6 stillbirths,  23 preterm  births) and  statistical analyses  have
not been completed.

Sydney
     Reports have  recently started to  emerge from a  longitudinal  study of
children in Sydney,  Australia (Cooney et al. , 1989a,  1989b;  McBride et al.,
1989).    Of an original cohort of 318, 298 mothers  and infants were  sampled for
blood lead levels at birth.  After 3 years,  215 children remained in the study.
A second cohort of 123 children was also recruited  because of "concern over the
possible contamination of  some of  the early capillary  blood  samples"  (Cooney
et al.,  1989b).   However,  the  results  reported by  Cooney et  al.  (1989a)
apparently pertain  to the original cohort  only.   Geometric mean blood  lead
levels  for mothers  and  infants at birth were  9.1  and 8.1  ug/dl, respectively
(overall range:   0-29  ug/dl).   Although  blood lead  measures were also taken at
6, 12,  18,  24,  30,  and 36 months  (McBride et al.,  1989), the postnatal values
were not considered in  the  analysis  of neurobehavioral outcomes during  the
first three years.   The  Bayley MDI and PDI  were administered at 6, 12,  and
24 months,  and  the  McCarthy  GCI  and Motor  Scales were  administered at
36 months.   Other outcomes were also  evaluated but the  results have not yet
been reported.
     Unadjusted  bivariate  correlations  between blood  lead levels  (either
maternal or  cord)  and cognitive and  psychomotor outcomes  (either Bayley  or
McCarthy Scales) were  generally small,  positive, and  nonsignificant  (Table 8).
The only statistically significant simple correlations were for the relation-
ship between cord blood  lead and  the 12-month Bayley MDI (r = 0.153, p <0.05,
two-tailed) and  PDI  (r = 0.167, p <0.05, two-tailed).  However, the direction
of the  relationship  was  positive,  i.e.,  as  blood lead increased, Bayley scores
increased.   After adjustment  for covariates,  the contribution of blood lead  to
the variance in the regression model approached significance only for the

3/27/89                              38

-------
                       PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
              TABLE 8.  REGRESSION OF
                             AND CORD
DEVELOPMENTAL INDICES ON MATERNAL
BLOOD LEAD LEVELS
Simple Correlations

6



12



24



36



Age
Months
Cognitive
Psychomotor

Months
Cognitive
Psychomotor

Months
Cognitive
Psychomotor

Months
Cognitive
Psychomotor

Maternal

-.044
.035


.015
.081


.006
.021


.040
.015

Cord

-.061
.025


.153*
.167*


.053
-.060


.045
.010

Effect of
Rz

.136
.153


.177
.106


.219
.081


.165
.081

F

3.
3.
df =

4.
2.
df =

4.
1.
df =

3.
1.
df =
Covariates


16
63
13

08
23
13

79
49
13

11
39
13
P

<.001
>.001
,261

<.001
<.01
,245

>.001
>.10
,222

<.001
>.10
,204
Incremental Effect
of Lead
Rz

.008
.003


.008
.019


.001
.013


.001
.001

F

1.22 >.
0.46 >.
df = 2,259

1.17 >.
2.60
df = 2,243

.19 >.
1.54 >.
df = 2,220

.13 >.
.10 >.
df = 2,202
P

25
60


30
08


70
20


70
90

*p <.05, two-tailed test.

Source:   Cooney et al.  (1989a).
    3/27/89
   39

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
positive  relationship  between  cord  blood  lead and  12-month Bayley  PDI
(p = 0.08, two-tailed).
     Regression analyses and path models indicated  that the greatest influences
on MDI  and  PDI  scores at  6  and  12 months were gestational  age and HOME  scores,
while at  later  ages  HOME  and parental  characteristics (maternal  IQ  and educa-
tion) were  the  more  important influences on cognitive  measures.  The fact that
Bayley  scores accounted for a significant amount of the variance in regressing
HOME scores on  prior HOME scores in two instances  (12-month  PDI  for  the 12-
versus  6-month  HOME  and 36-month MDI for the  36- versus 24-month  HOME) raises
the question of whether lead exposure  (e.g.,  maternal blood  lead) might have
covaried  with  HOME  scores.   This particular relationship  was  apparently not
examined  by the investigators.   However, the authors  did examine the relation-
ships between maternal/cord blood lead  and gestational age, which  were  shown to
be  statistically  nonsignificant  (as were  also birth weight,  obstetrical
complication, and  postnatal  risk factors).
     Cooney et al.  (1989b) reported the results  of  testing  the Sydney  cohort at
4 years of  age  on the McCarthy  GCI and  Motor Scales.   At 4 years, 207  children
of the original cohort remained  in the  study.   In addition  to  maternal  and cord
blood lead  samples at  delivery, postnatal  blood lead samples  were taken every
6 months.   Geometric means for capillary and venous blood samples  combined rose
from 15 ug/dl at 6 months  to a peak average of 16.4 ug/dl  at 18 months  and then
declined to 10.1 |jg/dl  at  48 months.
     Bivariate  and partial  (correcting  for  venous  versus capillary  collection)
correlations between 48-month McCarthy  Scales and  blood  lead  levels at differ-
ent ages  were  generally quite small, mixed in sign, and uniformly nonsignifi-
cant.   Analyses using composite  blood lead measurements (averaged over 12-month
periods)  produced  only one  significant relationship,  a  positive  correlation
(r = 0.160; p  <0.05, two-tailed) between  first  year blood  lead  and  GCI perfor-
mance (Table 9).  However,  after allowing for covariates,  regression analysis
showed  the  relationship to  be only marginally significant  (p  <0.07).   Analysis
of covariance  did not  indicate  that  change  in developmental outcome from 36 to
48 months was  significantly related  to  either current (48  month)  or cumulative
(current  and prior)  past  lead exposure, although cumulative lead was a better
predictor  (p  = 0.14)  than  current  lead exposure  alone (p = 0.36) for GCI
scores.    HOME  score  was stated  to be the  most important covariate  for  the  GCI,
apart from the  36-month GCI.

3/27/89                              40

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
          TABLE 9.  REGRESSION OF DEVELOPMENTAL INDICES AT 48 MONTHS
                       ON CURRENT AND PRIOR BLOOD LEVELS
Age
Prenatal :
Cognitive
Motor
First Year:
Cognitive
Motor
Second Year:
Cognitive
Motor
Third Year:
Cognitive
Motor
Fourth Year:
Cognitive
Motor
All Prior and Current:
Cognitive
Motor
Unadjusted
AR2 df
.004
.001
.023
.003
.001
.001
.001
.008
.001
.005
.044
.022
2,204
2,204
1,205
1,205
1,205
1,205
1,205
1,205
1,205
1,205
6,200
6,200
P
.36
.91
.03
.46
.82
.60
.70
.20
.88
.32
.17
.72
AR2
.009
.002
.013
.001
.002
.004
.003
.006
.001
.001
.028
.018
Adjusted
df
2,199
2,199
1,199
1,199
1,199
1,199
1,199
1,199
1,199
1,199
6,193
6,193
P
.14
.55
.07
.67
.60
.33
.76
.26
.76
.96
.14
.56
Source:  Cooney et al.  (1989b).


Mexico City
     Preliminary results of  a  pilot study in Mexico  City for a longitudinal
investigation  of  developmental outcomes  related  to lead exposure and  other
factors  have  been  reported  by  Rothenberg et  al.  (1989).   Approximately
50 mothers were sampled  for  blood lead levels  at 36  weeks  (M36)  of  pregnancy
and delivery (MD); umbilical  cord blood lead (UC) was also sampled at delivery.
Mean blood  lead levels  were:   M36, 15.0  MQ/dl;  MD,  15.4  ug/dl; and  UC,
13.8 ug/dl.   The  Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral  Assessment Scale  (NBAS)  was
administered to the infants at 48 hours and 15 and 30 days after birth.
     The  data  were analyzed by  calculating the trend of the NBAS subscale
scores over the first  30 days  by  linear  regression analysis  and  by  computing
the difference  in  M36  and  MD values or M36 and UC  values.  The relationships
among the various  primary  and  secondary measures were then examined through


3/27/89                              41

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR  CITE


bivariate  correlations  and  multivariate regression analyses  (Table  10).
Significant bivariate correlations were  found between  UC blood lead and  the
30-day trend in NBAS Abnormal Reflexes (r =  0.299,  p <0.05),  between the M36-MD
blood  lead  difference  and Regulation  of States (r =  0.378, p <0.05), and
between the  MD-UC  blood lead difference  and  Abnormal  Reflexes  (r = -0.451,
p <0.01).   The signs of all  the correlations  reflected impairment of function.
Stepwise multiple regression  modeling  with  all  covariates entered  before the
lead variable revealed that  the  blood lead  differentials for M36-MD  and for
MD-UC  accounted  for a significant  amount of  the  variance  in the  Abnormal
Reflexes trend (p ~ 0.03 for  each).   Similarly,  M36-MD  accounted  for a  signifi-
cant amount of the  variance  in Regulation of States (p = 0.025).   However,  UC
alone was no longer significantly associated with Abnormal  Reflexes.
     Alternatively  stated,  these  results reflect   impaired  neurobehavioral
outcomes in infants  as  the mothers'  blood lead levels  increased  from 36 weeks
to delivery or as  the  infants'  blood  lead  levels  approached or  exceeded the
mothers' at delivery.  Davis  and  Svendsgaard (1987) commented on  the possible
significance of  such disequilibria  in the  normal  maternal-fetal  blood  lead
relationship and the possibility  that  the fetus/infant could act as a  sink for
the mother's lead  burden.  However,  Rothenberg et   al.  (1989) speculated  that
some fetal  stress  factor  may be responsible  for both the disparity in fetal/
infant  lead burden  and the unfavorable neurobehavioral outcomes in  the neonate
(cf. Ernhart et al., 1986).
     Rothenberg et al.  (1989) also evaluated physical  development outcomes  at
birth  in  their cohort.   After controlling for covariates,  multiple regression
analyses  indicated  that  M36-MD and  M36-UC  each accounted for a significant
amount  of  the variance  in  birthweight  (p <0.05);   M36-UC  accounted for  a
significant amount of the variance in chest circumference (p = 0.054); and  UC
and M36-UC  accounted for  a significant amount of the variance in trunk length
(p s 0.06).

Yugoslavia
     A  longitudinal  study involving  two communities  in  Yugoslavia,  Titova
Mitrovica  and  Pristina,   has  been undertaken by Graziano et al.  (1989a,b).
T. Mitrovica  is  a  major lead smelter  and industrial  site,  whereas Pristina,
40 km  to  the  south,  serves as a  relatively  non-exposed control community.   The
analyses completed  thus  far  have been  retrospective as  well  as  prospective.
Only reproductive outcomes have been assessed.
3/27/89                              42

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     TABLE 10A.  SIGNIFICANT BIVARIATE CORRELATIONS OF 30-DAY NBAS* TREND
                               AND LEAD MEASURES
Maternal lead at 36 weeks (M36)

Maternal lead at delivery (MD)

Umbilical cord lead (UC)

M36 - MD

M36 - UC

MD - UC
           reflexes, 0.299**

           regulation of states, 0.378**
           reflexes, -0.451
                           ***
  *NBAS = Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale.
 **p <0.05.
***
        TABLE 10B.   EFFECT OF ADDITION OF LEAD TO THE STEPWISE MULTIPLE
     REGRESSION MODEL USING PRIOR ENTRY OF ALL CONTROL VARIABLES THAT HAVE
         SIGNIFICANT BIVARIATE CORRELATIONS WITH 30-DAY TREND OF NBAS*



NBAS Scale
Reflex

Regulation
of States


Lead
Measure
M36 - MD
MD - UC
M36 - MD




N
44
44
44



F
Value
4.87
5.08
5.54

Additional
Percent
of Variance
Explained
by Lead
6.0
6.2
8.5



P
Value
0.034
0.030
0.025

*NBAS habituation scale not tested because of low number of subjects with
 complete data.   All other variables not shown were not significant.

Source:   Rothenberg et al. (1989).
3/27/89
43

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE  OR CITE
     Of the first  1032  women  enrolled in the study,  639 (304 in T.  Mitrovica;
335 in Pristina) had  had  at least one pregnancy (only  first pregnancies were
considered in the  analysis) and had lived at their  current  address at least
since their first  pregnancy.   Geometric  mean blood lead levels  at the time of
initial interview were  15.9 ug/dl  in T.  Mitrovica and  5.1 ug/dl  in  Pristina.
The rate  of spontaneous abortions  (fetal  loss  prior to 7th month)  was  not
significantly different in the  two communities:  16.4%  of T. Mitrovica women
versus 14.0% of  Pristina  women reporting such loss.  Graziano  et al.  (1989b)
noted  that  they  had  systematically  attempted to demonstrate an  association
between lead exposure and  spontaneous abortion and  that  their failure to detect
such an association  suggested  that it did not exist at  the  levels of exposure
encountered in their samples.
     A preliminary  analysis  of prospective data from 907 births (401 in  T.
Mitrovica; 506 in  Pristina) indicated that mean birthweights did  not differ
significantly between the two  communities:   3308 g (SD  = 566)  in T.  Mitrovica
versus 3361 g  (SD  = 525)  in Pristina.   Geometric  mean  blood lead  levels were
17.1 ug/dl  in the  former and  5.1  ug/dl  in the  latter  mothers.   Regression
analysis controlling for several covariates also failed  to show any significant
relationship between mid-pregnancy blood lead levels and birthweight.

Glasgow
     Following a cross-sectional duplicate diet  study that showed,  among other
things, an  inverse relationship between gestational age and  lead exposure
during pregnancy  (Moore et al. ,  1982a), Moore  et al.  (1982b)  initiated  a
prospective study  of  the  neurobehavioral effects of lead in children born in
Glasgow, Scotland.   A major source of lead exposure for  this  population was its
plumbosolvent drinking  water.   However,  subsequent to a successful program to
control the plumbsolvency of  the water supply for Glasgow,  average blood lead
levels declined substantially (Richards and Moore,  1984).   Notwithstanding this
complication  in  the design  of their  prospective investigation, Moore et al.
(1989)  undertook  to  assess whether  prenatal  or perinatal  lead  exposure was
associated with birth outcomes or postnatal neurobehavioral  development.
     Their study sample consisted of 151 subjects drawn  from an initial pool of
885 families.   Based  on maternal  blood  lead levels during pregnancy,  three
groups, matched  for  social class, were  created:   high (£30 ug/dl,  mean  =
33.05), medium (15-25 ug/dl,  mean = 17.73), and low (S10 ug/dl, mean = 7.02).

3/27/89                              44

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


Infant blood  lead  levels  were measured at 1  and  2 years  of  age, but were  not
included in the reported analyses because of incomplete records.
     Although birth weight  appeared  to be inversely related to maternal blood
lead (a  reduction  of  nearly 100 g for each increment in blood lead grouping),
no statistical analysis of the data was reported.   Birth length showed a slight
trend in the  oppposite direction,  and other pediatric  measures  (head  circum-
ference, Apgar  scores, obstetric  complications)  showed no evident trend  in
either direction.
     Unadjusted Bayley  scores  (MDI,  PDI,  and mean) at 1 and 2 years generally
decreased with  increasing maternal blood  lead grouping.   However, stepwise
linear regression  anlayses  indicated  that  birth weight, social class,  and  HOME
scores accounted for  Bayley Scales performance better than  lead exposure  (as
represented by  maternal blood  lead,  water lead  concentration,  or reported
history of pica).   Since  birth weight was significantly  related inversely to
lead exposure, it was removed from the model  to see if the explanatory power of
one of the  lead  variables could be improved.  Only  second year  pica showed a
notable  improvement,  although HOME score  alone   still  had less predictive
ability  for  2-year Bayley  scores  than pica  coupled with HOME  score.   An
analysis of  the  Bayley IBR  revealed  no  consistent direction of association
between  lead  exposure and IBR  factors.  Moore et al.  (1989) concluded  that
their dataset provided  "no  firm evidence for either a  direct or an indirect
contribution of lead to decrements in cognitive development."

Christchurch
     Fergusson and his colleagues (Fergusson  and Purchase,  1987;  Fergusson
et al.,  1988a,b,c)  collected  shed  deciduous teeth from  more  than 1000  children
in Christchurch, New  Zealand  to assess their long-term lead exposure  and  the
relationship of such  exposure to neurobehavioral  outcomes.  Blood lead levels
were not measured.
     As reported by Fergusson et al.  (1988b), IQ, reading ability, and school
performance were  assessed in relation to  dentine lead levels in  samples  of
664-886 children from  an  original  cohort of 1265 drawn from the Christchurch
Child Development  Study.  The subjects were evaluated  at  ages 8  and 9  years on
the WISC-R, the Burt Reading Test, and by teachers'  ratings of reading, written
expression, spelling,  mathematics, and handwriting.   All bivariate correlations
between these outcomes  and  dentine lead  levels were  in  the predicted direction

3/27/89                              45

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
(negative), and  all  but  one  (out of 18) were statistically  significant  at
p <0.05 (one-tailed).  After  correcting  for test unreliability, dentine  lead
measurement error,  sample  selection  factors,  and  several  covariates, the
coefficients for the  reading  test and all  five teacher ratings at both ages 8
and 9 years remained significant.   Further  correction for pica (to test for the
reverse causality  hypothesis  that reduced  cognitive ability  results  in more
pica, which in turn  results in greater lead exposure) reduced the size of the
correlations even more  (ranging from  -0.07  to  -0.14),  but 7/12  of  the  correla-
tions remained statistically significant at p <0.05 (Table 11).

  TABLE 11.  UNEXPLAINED CORRELATIONS BETWEEN DENTINE LEAD LEVELS (log
         TAKING INTO ACCOUNT TEST  RELIABILITY,  CONFOUNDING COVARIATES,
           SAMPLE SELECTION FACTORS, AND REVERSE CAUSALITY VIA PICA
Measure
Verbal IQ
Performance IQ
Total IQ
Burt reading test
Teacher ratings
Reading
Written expression
Spelling
Mathematics
Handwriting
8 years
r
-0.03
-0.02
-0.04
-0.07

-0.13
-0.13
-0.14
-0.08
-0.12
P*
N.S.
N.S.
N.S.
<0.05

<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
<0.10
<0.05
9 years
r
-0.02
-0.02
-0.03
-0.08

-0.08
-0.08
-0.09
-0.10
-0.08
P*
N.S.
N.S.
N.S.
<0.05

<0.10
N.S.
<0.10
<0.05
<0.10
*0ne-tailed test.
Source:  Fergusson et al.  (1988b).

     Fergusson  et al.  (1988c)  also  investigated  the hypothesis  that the
demonstrated relationship between  lead  exposure  and  school  performance was due
to  lead-related  deficits  in  attentional  processes.   Mothers' and  teachers'
ratings of  signs  of  restless activity and  inattention  at ages 8 and 9 years
were correlated  with  dentine lead levels of 888 children (Table 12).  Several
aspects of  the  ratings  were significantly  correlated with  dentine lead,  with
bivariate correlations  for  total  rating scores ranging between 0.08 (p <0.05)
and  0.14  (p <0.01,  one-tailed).   Teachers'  ratings were more highly correlated
with lead and  were  also judged  to  be a  more accurate measure of  the children's
behavior than were maternal  ratings.  After correcting for measurement errors,

3/27/89                              46

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
       TABLE 12.   PRODUCT MOMENT CORRELATIONS BETWEEN MATERNAL,  TEACHER
             BEHAVIOUR RATINGS AND DENTINE LEAD VALUES (log M9/g)

                                          8 years                9 years
Measure                                r           p*         r            p*

Maternal ratings

Activity
 Restless, overactive                 0.07       <0.05       0.05       <0.10
 Excitable, impulsive                 0.07       <0.05       0.05       <0.10
 Constantly fidgeting                 0.03       N.S.         0.04       N.S.
 Always climbing                      0.07       <0.05       0.04       N.S.
 Squirmy, fidgety                     0.03       N.S.         0.06       <0.05

Attention
 Short attention span                 0.09       <0.01       0.08       <0.01
 Inattentive, easily distracted       0.09       <0.01       0.05       <0.10
 Can't settle to tasks                0.06       <0.05       0.04       N.S.

Total Score                           0.11       <0.01       0.08       <0.05

Teacher ratings

Activity
 Restless, overactive                 0.09       <0.01       0.11       <0.001
 Excitable, impulsive                 0.03       N.S.         0.10       <0.001
 Squirmy, fidgety                     0.11       <0.001      0.13       <0.001
 Very restless                        0.10       <0.001      0.13       <0.001

Attention
 Inattentive, easily distracted       0.16       <0.001      0.14       <0.001
 Short attention span                 0.11       <0.001      0.12       <0.001
 Poor concentration                   0.13       <0.001      0.12       <0.001

Total Score                           0.13       <0.001      0.14       <0.001

"'One-tailed test.

Source:  Fergusson et al.  (1988c).


sample selection factors,  covariates,  and pica, the correlation  between dentine
lead and inattention/restless behavior was 0.08 (p <0.01) at both 8 and 9 years

of age.

     Although  small,  the corrected  correlations between  dentine lead  and

various outcomes reflecting  school  behavior and performance were consistent and

stable over a  one-year interval  in the  Christchurch  study.   The  uniformity  and

stability of these  results  across  time provide compelling reason to judge the

effects as real.   However,  this  same study provided no evidence that intelli-
3/27/89                              47

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE  OR  CITE


gence, as measured by the WISC-R at 8 or 9 years  of  age,  was  related to  dentine
lead levels.
     The lack of  blood  lead  measurements makes it difficult to interpret the
Christchurch study for  dose-response  information.   However,  the mean dentine
lead value in this  study was 6 ug/g, which may be compared to a mean dentine
lead level of ~14.5  ug/g found by Needleman et al.  (1979; see also Bellinger
et al., 1984b) for a sample  of 2335 Boston children.   Given  the comparability
of the methods employed by Fergusson (Fergusson  and Purchase,  1987;  Fergusson
et al., 1988a,b,c) and  by  Needleman et  al. (1979), and an estimated mean blood
lead level on the order of 30  ug/dl at  age 2-3 years in  the study by Needleman
et al.  (1979), it is possible to infer that the average blood lead  level  of  the
Christchurch cohort  could  have been roughly on the order of $15 ug/dl at age
2-3 years.   Although this  estimate  is rather imprecise and based on  several
assumptions,  it suggests that  the  level of lead exposure in the Christchurch
study population  was  roughly  comparable to populations  in some  of  the  other
studies under consideration here.

Nordenham
     As noted in  the 1986 Addendum, Winneke et al.  (1985a,b)  enrolled 114 chil-
dren in 1982  from a  population of 383  children  born  6-7 years previously in
Nordenham,  Federal Republic  of Germany.   An ongoing federal  screening program
of the Nordenham  residents had taken blood samples from mothers and cords  at
delivery.   The maternal  geometric  mean  blood lead level  was  9.3 |jg/dl  (range:
4-31); for  umbilical  cord, it was 8.2  ug/dl  (range:  4-30).   When tested at
age 6-7,  the  children's average blood  lead  level  was again 8.2 ug/dl,  but
distributed differently  (range: 4-23).   In terms of accounting for a signifi-
cant amount  of  variance  in  various measures of reaction time performance,
maternal  blood lead  was better than cord;  the  combination of maternal and cord
blood lead was better than either  alone;  and  the combination was about  as good
as concurrent blood  lead.   These  relative standings are probably  only  rough
comparisons, since differences  in  the quality of the  samples  from  different
sources and  at  different  times could  have affected these  results  (Winneke
et al., 1985a).
     Retesting of 76 of the Nordenham children at age 9,  with blood lead levels
then averaging 7.8 ug/dl  (range:  4-21), indicated some persisting deficits in
reaction time test performance related  to blood lead  levels  3 years earlier

3/27/89                              48

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
(Winneke et al.,  1989a,b).   WISC-R performance was also significantly related
to  the preceding  blood  lead  levels  after correction  for confounding.
Concurrent blood lead did not significantly account for either reaction time or
WISC-R performance at  age 9, although  it  had (for  reaction  time  errors)  at  age
6-7.   No  results were  reported  for the relationship of  9-year  outcomes to
perinatal  blood lead measures.

Buffalo
     A prospective  study was recently  initiated in  the Buffalo, NY  area by
Shucard et al.  (1988a,b).   Cord  blood levels averaged ~4.4 ug/dl for 802 new-
borns.  Outcomes have not yet been reported.

Other Recent Studies
     In addition  to  the longitudinal studies discussed above,  several  other
studies of neurobehavioral function in lead-exposed children have been reported
since the 1986  Addendum.   Although most appear to be well  conducted, many  of
these  studies  are of  somewhat  limited relevance,  either because of their
cross-sectional design,  their  relatively  high  blood lead  levels,  or their
primary reliance on  tooth lead  as an exposure indicator.   Despite its limita-
tions, blood  lead is  currently  the bioindicator  of greatest  utility  for
regulatory decision-making purposes.   Consequently,  the following studies will
be discussed in less detail than the prospective longitudinal studies described
above.
     A cross-sectional  study of  cognitive abilities  and educational  attainment
in a  population  of  school-age  children from central Edinburgh,  Scotland has
been  reported by  Fulton et al.  (1987)  and  Raab et al.  (1989).   The  geometric
mean blood lead  level  for the 501 children in the  study sample was 11.5 ug/dl
(range:  3.3-34.0).    Multiple  regression  analyses  indicated  significant
relationships between log-transformed blood lead levels and composite scores on
the British  Ability Scales  (p  =  0.003)  and  between blood  lead  levels  and
attainment test  scores for quantitative (p =  0.04)  and reading  (p = 0.001)
skills, even after  allowing  for  33 covariates.  Grouping  the subjects by blood
lead  levels  showed  a  clear  dose-response  relationship without  any evident
threshold down to the lowest subgroup mean blood lead level of 5.6 ug/dl.
     Hatzakis et  al.   (1987, 1989) conducted neuropsychological  testing on
509 children living  near a lead smelter in Lavrion,  Greece, and found impair-
ments in WISC-R  IQ  scores and reaction time performance scores.   These  effects
3/27/89                              49

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
were significantly associated with  blood lead levels after controlling for as
many as 23 covariates in multiple regression models.   Blood lead concentrations
ranged from 7.4 to 63.9 ug/dl and averaged 23.7 ug/dl.   Depending on the number
of covariates  included  in  the model,  full  scale  IQ decreased by  2.4-2.7  points
for every 10-ug/dl increase in blood lead concentration.   Subjects were grouped
by blood  lead  levels (10-|jg/dl  increments) to analyze dose-response relation-
ships for  IQ  as  well as reaction performance  scores.   A nonlinearity in the
dose-response pattern for  IQ makes  it difficult to interpret these data for a
threshold.  However,  reaction time performance showed no evident threshold.
     Wolf  et  al.  (1987)  used  structural equation modeling  to  evaluate the
variables  related to low-level  lead exposure and infant mental  development in
an  urban  population  in  Costa  Rica.  Blood  lead  levels  averaged 10.8 ug/dl
(range: 5.4-21.5  ug/dl) in  182  subjects whose average  age  was 16.6 months
(range: 12-23  months).   Blood  lead  had neither direct nor indirect effects on
the Bayley MDI.   However,  blood  lead  did have  a  significant  negative relation-
ship with birth weight (p = 0.05) and mother's height (p = 0.001), as well as a
positive  relationship  with mother's  age (p = 0.01),  after adjustment  for
covariates.  Birth weight, iron deficiency anemia, and child's age had signifi-
cant direct effects on MDI.
     Vivoli et al. (1989)  measured  lead concentrations  in blood,  teeth, and
hair, as  well  as  ALA-D  activity,  in 237 children  from  Sassuolo,  Italy.   At the
time of the study, blood lead levels averaged ~11.5 |jg/dl.   Although concurrent
blood  lead measures  showed no significant relationship  to covariate-adjusted
scores  on any of  six neurobehavioral  tests,  tooth  lead was significantly
related to full  scale and verbal WISC-R scores.   Moreover, ALA-D was signifi-
cantly  associated with one  verbal  subtest  of the WISC-R and with  delayed
reaction time performance.
     Tooth lead  also accounted for a significant amount of the  variance in
WISC-R  full  scale and verbal IQ as well  as  Bender Gestalt visual motor test
performance  in 156  children  from Aarhus, Denmark  (Hansen et  al.,  1989).
Circumpupal  tooth lead levels  averaged 10.7 ug/g.  A  study  of children  in
Brussels  (Cluydts and  Steenhout,  1989)  showed marginally significant (p <0.10)
covariate-adjusted regression coefficients  for tooth lead and neurobehavioral
outcomes  (WISC-R, reaction time and attentional performance), despite the  small
(N = 41)  number of children evaluated.
3/27/89                              50

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     In separate  but  similar studies conducted in Rhode  Island  and Lavrion,
Greece, Faust  and Brown (1987) and Benetou-Marantidou et  al.  (1988),  respec-
tively, administered  neurobehavioral  test batteries to small  groups  (N's of
15-30) of  children  whose blood lead levels had been measured at ~30-60 ug/dl.
Significantly  impaired  performance  was  evident by comparison  to  matched  con-
trols, even  after blood lead levels had  been  below  30 ug/dl  for  at least one
year  (Faust  and  Brown,  1987)  or after  a 4-year intervening  period  before
follow-up testing (Benetou-Marantidou et al., 1988).
     In analyses of data from the second National  Health and Nutrition Examina-
tion  Survey,  Schwartz and Otto (1987)  have  also  found evidence  of retarded
neurobehavioral development and impaired neurosensory capability in relation to
low-level   lead  exposure.   The  ages  at which a child first sat up, walked, and
spoke were significantly associated with blood lead levels, as was also hearing
threshold.    The decline in hearing ability  is  qualitatively  consistent with
other evidence  linking  low-level  lead exposure to  electrophysiological  changes
in the auditory system (reviewed in U.S.  EPA, 1986a).
     With  regard  to fetal  growth  effects,  a  recent report by Ward  et al.
(1987) indicated  that placental  lead concentrations were highly significantly
correlated with  reductions  in  birth weight,  head circumference,  and placental
weight in  100  obstretically normal  births in England.   Gestational  age  was
correlated at  borderline statistical significance  (p <0.10).  Dividing  the
data into  two  birthweight  groups,  low (<3000  g) and high  (>4000  g),  revealed
a  highly   significant  difference  in  mean placental  lead  concentrations:
2.349 ug/9 (S.D.  =  0.883)  versus  1.122 pg/g  (S.D. = 0.361) for low and high
weight groups,  respectively.   Of  the 37 elements analyzed,  lead  and cadmium
showed the most consistent  negative relationships  with these  fetal  outcomes.
Other factors,  such as  parity, sex of neonate, social  class,  and history of
miscarriage,  did  not  appear to the authors  to significantly  confound  their
results.

Conclusions

     As noted in the 1986 Addendum (U.S.  EPA, 1986b), prospective studies offer
a major advantage  over  cross-sectional  and even many retrospective studies in
that they  provide  a better history of lead  exposure.  This key difference is
the reason why more weight  is placed on findings  from prospective studies.

3/27/89                              51

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
However, notwithstanding this advantage, prospective  studies  may have various
types of  shortcomings  in common  with other  epidemiological  studies.  One
problem of  particular  importance  is statistical power, especially  in longi-
tudinal  studies that typically experience attrition and declining sample sizes
over the course of the study.
     The studies reviewed here differ considerably in  population size and,  even
within some studies, in the  number of subjects included in individual analyses.
To illustrate,  the  first results  from the  Boston prospective  study  were based
on analyses involving  201 infants out of a cohort of 249 (Bellinger et al.,
1987a).   By 5 years, the analyses  were based on 170 children (Bellinger et  al.,
1989b).   While  this rate of attrition  is certainly not high,  it  does neverthe-
less make  it  increasingly difficult  to  detect an effect of  low-level  lead
exposure.    According  to Cohen (1977),  an  N  of over  400  subjects would be
required to detect  an  effect  size of 0.01  with a power of 0.80 at an alpha of
0.05, one-sided.   This calculation assumes  that the  analysis  includes 13
covariates  having an  R2  of  0.30.   If the covariate R2 is  overestimated in  the
analysis for some reason, e.g.,  because of  spurious sample correlations between
covariates  and  blood  lead or because  the  covariates  are  involved in  causal
pathways linking lead exposure to  development, then regression methods may  fail
to detect  a truly  significant  association between  lead  and developmental
outcomes.
     Another factor that can  affect the  power to detect a signficant relation-
ship in  these  studies  is  the amount  of variance in  the  sample blood  lead
measures.    A  small  standard deviation  in the  independent  variable (e.g., blood
lead) will  necessarily  reduce any correlation  between it  and  a dependent
variable (e.g., MDI scores).   Also, the presence of  a  significant  effect  of
another variable on the outcomes  under consideration will make it more diffi-
cult to detect  the  effect of lead exposure by regression  analysis.   For exam-
ple, over  50 percent  of the mothers  enrolled in the Cleveland study  were
determined  to be  alcoholic,  and  significant early developmental  effects were
shown to be alcohol-related  in the Cleveland  study population (Ernhart et  al.,
1985b).
     Perhaps the most surprising outcome of the prospective studies   as a whole,
then, is that so  many of them are able  to detect any effect of lead at all.
Given the  limitations  of sample  size  and power of most of the studies under
consideration,  one's  confidence in the reality  of any  detected effects that

3/27/89                              52

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
achieve statistical  significance  is  enhanced.   Conversely, it is difficult to
interpret  a  failure to  detect a  lead  effect  as suggesting the absence  of
effect, if the  analyses  in question were based on fewer than 400 subjects, as
was generally the case.
     Despite  these  limitations, some  important conclusions emerge  from  the
findings  reported above.   Various lines of  evidence still  relate  neuro-
behavioral effects to  blood lead levels of "10-15 ug/dl, and possibly lower,"
as was  previously concluded in the 1986 Addendum  (U.S.  EPA,  1986b).   Further
analyses  from the Boston study, which  has provided the  most direct information
bearing on dose-response  relationships  for  neurobehavioral effects,  not  only
supported the 10-15  |jg/dl  level of concern  but indicated  that MDI deficits can
be detected  in  relation  to cord blood  lead  levels of 6-7 (jg/dl  in  lower SES
children  (Bellinger  et al.,  1988).   Since the Boston cohort was  mostly middle
to upper-middle class, "lower"  SES merely refers to less than the highest SES
levels  and is probably in fact much closer  to the median of the U.S.  popula-
tion than  the term suggests.   Although  the  postnatal  lead exposure levels were
somewhat  higher in  the Port Pirie study, analyses of the relationship between
postnatal   blood  lead levels  and  covariate-adjusted MDI scores provided  "no
evidence  of  a threshold  effect" (Wigg et al.,  1988).   Indeed, restricting the
analysis  to  children with  blood lead levels below 25 ug/dl  in the Port  Pirie
study yielded an  even  stronger association  between covariate-adjusted  McCarthy
GCI scores  and  integrated postnatal blood  lead measures (McMichael  et  al.,
1988).
     Supporting  evidence for  the  stated level  of  concern  may also be  derived
from other studies.  Although McCarthy Scale results from the Boston  study have
not been  analyzed in  a  manner to allow direct  extraction  of dose-response
information,   the  average  blood lead  level  significantly  associated  with
covariate-adjusted performance  on  the  McCarthy Scales  (GCI  and Perceptual-
Performance  subscale)  was  6.8  ug/dl  (SD =  6.3) (Bellinger  et al.,  1987b).
Similarly, analyses  relating  cord  blood lead levels  to the  G-R  Neurological
Soft Signs and,  indirectly, to 12-month MDI  scores were  previously reported for
the Cleveland study  (Ernhart  et al., 1986; Wolf et al.,  1985), along with more
recent significant results, by more than one analysis, relating cord  blood lead
to Length  of Utterance,  a measure of  language  development  in 24-month-old
infants (Morrow-Tlucak and Ernhart, 1987).   Significant  relationships were also
indicated for maternal blood  lead  and deficits  in  6-month  MDI,  PDI, and KID

3/27/89                              53

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE


scores  in  the Cleveland  cohort,  but results  of other analyses were mixed
(Ernhart et al.,  1987a).   Any significant neurobehavioral effects associated
with  cord  blood lead in  the  Cleveland  study necessarily occurred at levels
below 15 ug/dl since the maximum single cord blood lead level  measured was only
14.7 ng/dl  (mean:  5.99 pg/dl).  Moreover, the fact that significant evidence of
lead-associated impairments was  found  in a relatively small  cohort (N's <200)
suggests that  the work was  carefully conducted  and  that the  results  are
credible.
     Preliminary evidence from the Mexico City study is not inconsistent with a
level of concern  of 10-15 ug/dl,  and possibly  lower.   Rothenberg et al.  (1989)
found that two  measures  of neonatal  neurobehavioral function during the first
30 days of life were significantly accounted for by differentials  in maternal
blood lead levels  during  the last month of pregnancy and/or maternal  and cord
blood lead levels.  The nature of the reported analyses does not provide direct
dose-response information, but  the mean cord blood lead  level  in  the Mexico
City  cohort  was 13.8 ug/dl  and  the mean  maternal  delivery blood lead  was
15.4 pg/dl, levels  that  approximate the stated level of  concern.  However,
little weight can  be placed  on these findings until more complete results are
obtained from a larger number of subjects.
     Some evidence  from  recent  cross-sectional  studies  is  also  consistent
with  the  identified level of concern.   The  Edinburgh study (e.g.,  Fulton
et al.,  1987)  shows a  significant relationship  between  blood  lead  levels
averaging as  low as  5.6  ug/dl and  covariate-adjusted scores of  cognitive
ability and educational attainment.   As noted by Grant and Davis (1989), this
finding appears to  closely parallel  the results of Schroeder and Hawk (1987),
whose North  Carolina cross-sectional  study population  showed IQ  deficits in
relation to blood lead extending to levels as below 10 ug/dl.   The latter study
was  described  in  detail  and  evaluated  in  the  1986 Air Quality Criteria for
Lead  (U.S.  EPA,  1986a).   In  addition,  the  reaction  time performance  results  of
Hatzakis et al.  (1989)  showed no evident threshold over a blood lead range of
7.4 to 63.9 ug/dl.
     Based on  all  of  the above  considerations,  a  blood lead concentration
of 10-15 ug/dl,  and possibly lower,  remains the  level of concern for impaired
neurobehavioral development  in  infants  and children.   Given  the fact  that such
effects  have  been  associated with blood  lead measures  in pregnant  women,
umbilical  cords,  and infants up to at least 2 years  of  age,  there  is no

3/27/89                              54

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
apparent distinction at  present  as  to whether  this  level  of  concern  applies  to
only fetuses or infants or preschool-age children.   Thus,  a blood lead level  of
10-15 ug/dl, and  possibly lower, ought to be  avoided in pregnant  women,
fetuses, infants, and  young  children,  although it is recognized that pregnant
women per se are not necessarily a population at risk.
     Various lines of evidence suggest that lower SES and male gender are addi-
tional  risk  factors  for  the  developmental effects of low-level lead exposure.
Increased vulnerability  in lower SES children has  been  indicated  in analyses
of MDI  scores  (Bellinger et  al.,  1988)  and MDI-GCI  change scores in  the  Boston
study  (Bellinger  et al.,  1989b) and in  the MDI  results from  Cincinnati
(Dietrich et al., 1987b,  1989b).   Also, some  IQ  results  from cross-sectional
investigations are consistent  with  the  view that lower SES  children are more
vulnerable to  lead-induced cognitive impairment  (Harvey et al.,  1984;  Lansdown
et al.,  1986;  Schroeder  and Hawk,   1987).   Greater susceptibility  of male
infants to  lead developmental  toxicity  has been  evident  in analyses  of MDI-GCI
change  scores  in  the Boston  study  (Bellinger et  al.,  1989b), in MDI results
from the Cincinnati  study (Dietrich et  al.,  1987b,  1989b), in reanalyses of  IQ
data from the  Southampton cross-sectional  study (Pocock et al., 1987), and in
some early  data on  sex ratios  of stillbirths  in  Port Pirie and other locations
(Scragg et al., 1977).
     The evidence  regarding  pregnancy  outcomes  and physical  growth effects
related to  prenatal  lead exposure is less  consistent than that for neurobehav-
ioral outcomes.  Such  was the  case  at the  time of the  1986 Addendum  (U.S.  EPA,
1986b)  and  is  still the case simply because  little additional  information
pertaining to pregnancy outcomes and growth has appeared since the 1986 assess-
ment.
     As far  as  growth  effects  are concerned, the Cincinnati  study has shown a
significant  covariate-adjusted reduction  in birth  weight associated with
prenatal (maternal)  blood lead levels (Bornschein et al., 1989).   In addition,
early postnatal (3-15  months)  growth rates have  also been associated with lead
exposure pre-  and postnatally  in the Cincinnati study  (Shukla et  al., 1987).
(Interactions with other  variables  were evident in both of these cases,  which
will  be discussed further below.)
     Some supporting evidence  of lead-related reductions in birth weight also
comes from the Boston and Mexico City studies and, possibly,  the Glasgow study.
Although birth weight  per ^e showed no  relationship to cord blood lead in the

3/27/89                              55

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Boston study, there was  an exposure-related trend in the percentage of small-
for-gestational  age infants that approached statistical  significance (Bellinger
et al., 1984a).   In Mexico City, the difference  in the blood lead level of the
mother at 36  weeks  versus  either her level  at delivery  (M36-MD) or  the blood
lead  level  of the cord  (M36-UC) accounted for a significant amount of  the
variance  in birth  weight   (Rothenberg  et  al.,  1989).    Similar  findings
(p = 0.06) were noted  for  chest circumference and trunk  length.  In Glasgow,
the  high  maternal  blood  lead group (mean:  33.05  ug/dl)  weighed 3.32 kg, on
average,  at  birth,  whereas the medium  blood lead group (mean:  17.73 ug/dl)
weighed 3.43  kg and the  low group  (mean:  7.02 ug/dl) weighed 3.51 kg (Moore et
al., 1989).    However,  these data were not adjusted for covariates and were not
tested for statistical  significance.
     Otherwise,  no other prospective study has shown a significant  association
between  reduced  birth weight  and  lead  exposure.   The  Yugoslavian  study
(Graziano et  al.,  1989b),   in particular,  has  failed thus far  to yield  any
evidence of lead-related birthweight reductions  in more than 900  births,  even
at relatively high  blood lead levels.   Also, analyses by Ernhart et  al. (1986)
showed no significant  effect of lead on  birth weight,  birth length, or head
circumference; nor, according to preliminary analyses,  was any  effect evident
on postnatal  growth (Marler and Ernhart, 1987).   However, the cross-sectional
analysis by Ward et al. (1987) did  indicate highly significant simple relation-
ships between placenta!  lead concentrations and  reduced birth weight and head
circumference.
     The  Port  Pirie study  provided  strong  evidence relating prenatal  lead
exposure  to  increased  risk  of  preterm  delivery  (<37 weeks  gestation)  in a
sample of 749 pregnancies (McMichael  et al., 1986).   Also, a small  but signifi-
cant relationship between  prenatal  lead and gestational  maturity was observed
in  the  structural  analyses  of  the  Cincinnati  study (e.g.,  Dietrich et al.,
1987b).  However,  regression analyses of a different sample from the Cincinnati
study  did not reveal  a  significant association  between prenatal  lead and
preterm  (<35  weeks) deliveries  (Bornschein et  al.,  1989).   Also, as previously
discussed in  the  1986  Addendum, the  Boston prospective  study showed a positive
but  nonsignificant  relationship between cord blood lead  and gestation length
(Bellinger et al.,  1984a), whereas the  cross-sectional  study of Moore et al.
(1982a)  found signficant  negative associations  between  gestational  age  and
maternal  as well  as cord blood  lead  after allowing for  a number of  covariates.

3/27/89                              56

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Placental  lead  concentrations appeared to be  inversely  related to gestation
length  in  the  analyses of Vimpani et  al.  (1989)  and Ward et al. (1987), but
were of uncertain statistical significance.
     There are  many  possible explanations for these  apparent  inconsistencies
among  studies.   The  lack  of a  significant difference in  pregnancy  outcomes
between  the  high exposure and reference  communities  in  Yugoslavia (Graziano
et al., 1989a,b)  could reflect  the difficulty of  assessing fetal lead exposure
by means  of maternal  blood lead levels.   With regard to  gestation length,
Bornschein et al.  (1989)  noted  that their analysis was restricted to pregnan-
cies of at least 35 weeks.  A similar cutoff of 34 weeks  was used in the Boston
prospective  study  (Bellinger et al., 1984a).  Constraining  the data in this
fashion could make  it more difficult to detect an effect on gestation.  Also,
there were  interactions  involving mother's age and race  evident in the Cincin-
nati  study (Bornschein et al.,  1989).  Thus,  differences  in the age  of the
mothers, racial  make-up,  other  population characteristics, sample sizes, level
of lead  exposure (current as well as  past),  and  approaches to analyzing data
could underlie the varying results of different studies.
     Based on the  evidence reviewed here and in the 1986 Air Quality Criteria
for Lead (U.S.  EPA, 1986a), it seems likely that prenatal  lead exposure poses a
potential hazard to the developing fetus in terms of reduced gestational length
and possibly other  aspects of fetal growth  (in addition to postnatal neuro-
behavioral  development, as  already noted  above).   It  is  difficult,  however,  to
derive  a  definitive  dose-response  relationship  for fetal outcomes  from the
available data,  although  some indications point to a  level of  concern starting
in the  region of 10-15 |jg/dl.  The  average  maternal  blood lead levels in the
studies where  the pre-term  delivery effect was  clearest (Port  Pirie and
Glasgow) was  in the 10-15 yg/dl  range, with somewhat mixed findings  in the
Cincinnati, Boston, and Cleveland studies where the maternal or cord blood lead
levels  averaged  below 10  pg/dl.   A  similar  pattern seems to  hold for birth
weight as well.   The  strongest  evidence of a birthweight effect comes from the
Cincinnati  study, with some of  their analyses suggesting  that such  an effect
could start  in  the  region of 12-13 (jg/dl, but possibly extending from 7 to 18
ug/dl.  However, other prospective studies provide no support  for this conclu-
sion,  and  so it must, be considered  an open  issue for more definitive resolu-
tion.
3/27/89                              57

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     The issue of  the  persistence  of the neurobehavioral effects needs to be
considered  in  an assessment of  the risk of  low-level  lead  exposure.   If
decreased scores on  the  Bayley Scales  reflected merely  a transient  delay in
children's  neurobehavioral development -- a  minor perturbation that children
could  quickly  "grow  out  of"  — then the  public  health significance of lead
exposure at blood lead levels of 10-15  ug/dl  would perhaps be  diminished.   Some
recent results from  two  of the studies that had  convincingly demonstrated a
link between prenatal lead exposure (either  maternal  or cord blood lead levels)
and early deficits on  the  Bayley Mental Development  Index now suggest  that the
association between  prenatal lead  exposure  and cognitive development  may  not
hold up at  later ages.   The  Cincinnati study  found  a  declining influence of
prenatal exposure  indicators on  MDI scores  at 12  months and  24 months:  only
10-day blood lead  measures were  significantly associated with MDI  scores  at
12 months, and no significant negative  PbB-MDI  relationships were evident at 24
months (Dietrich et  al.,  1989b).   Although  the Boston  study  did  continue to
find a significant  relationship between  prenatal  lead exposure (cord blood
lead)  and MDI  performance at 24 months,  no  relationship could be shown  for
prenatal lead  exposure and cognitive abilities at 57  months  on  the McCarthy
Scales  (Bellinger  et al., 1987b).    Thus, one  implication  of  these 'findings
might be that the effects of  prenatal lead exposure on  neurobehavioral  develop-
ment are not permanent.
     Such a conclusion could  be valid,  but the evidence available to support it
is not  adequate.   The  inability  to detect a continuing significant association
between prenatal lead  exposure and neurobehavioral function  at age  2  in the
Cincinnati  study or  age  5 in the  Boston  study could result from, among other
things, a lack of  adequate statistical power, as  noted above.  Other  factors
could  also  interfere with  detecting such a relationship and  possibly  account
for differing  results  in separate  studies.  For  example, it is not clear  which
measure of  blood lead  provides the best indicator of exposure during critical
periods of  organogenesis.  The Cincinnati study primarily used maternal  blood
samples obtained during  the  first  or second trimester  of pregnancy  to  indicate
prenatal lead  exposure.   Prenatal  exposure was also  reflected in  samples  taken
from the  Cincinnati  infants  10 days after birth  (gestationally corrected),  but
comparatively  few  cord blood samples were available for data analyses.   The
Boston  study relied  exclusively  on cord  blood  samples  while other studies have
also  sampled  the mother's blood around  the  time of delivery.  As  suggested

3/27/89                              58

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
previously  (U.S.  EPA,  1986b; Davis  and  Svendsgaard,  1987) and supported by
preliminary results from the Mexico City study (Rothenberg et al.,  1989),  there
may be  differences  in  these  measures, particularly during  the  last month or so
of pregnancy,  that reflect  important  biokinetic transfers  of lead  between
compartments, both within the mother and infant individually and between them.
     In addition to the  nature  and extent of such biokinetic transfers, their
timing  (and  the  point  at which  blood lead is sampled) could make an  important
difference  in  the  ontogenesis of neural structures and,  consequently,  later
neurobehavioral function.  It is  interesting to  note,  for  example, that almost
no significant negative  associations  have  been found, after covariate adjust-
ment,  between concurrent blood lead levels  and postnatal  outcomes in  any of  the
prospective  studies  reviewed here.   Rather,  cumulative  past exposure (e.g.,
average postnatal  blood  lead levels)  or,  in several  instances, blood  lead
levels  several months  or years  prior to  a  given  outcome  have shown the  strong-
est relationship to postnatal neurobehavioral effects.  Such  a  lagged  effect
has been suggested by several findings,  as  shown in Table 13.  When significant
positive (unpredicted) relationships between a  measure of blood lead and  some
outcome have occasionally  been  found, they generally  involved  concurrent blood
lead levels.
     Thus,  it may well  be that the ability to detect a significant  relationship
between low-level  lead exposure and  neurobehavioral outcomes depends, at least
in part, on  where  and  when the measure  of blood lead is obtained.   Since the
fetus  and  infant constitute  the  population  at  greatest risk, it would be
preferable  to  measure  a direct indicator  of their exposure.  However, that
approach has not been  as feasible as measuring  a  related  indicator, such  as
maternal or  cord blood  lead.   (Even cord  blood  is sampled from  the  placenta!
rather than the fetal  side and may therefore not fully reflect fetal  exposure.)
Consequently,  measures  such  as  maternal  and cord blood  lead  levels, while
reasonably good  indicators of prenatal  lead exposure, may not afford the  most
accurate predictors of later neurobehavioral outcomes.
     Another factor that could  obscure  a  relationship between prenatal lead
exposure and postnatal  neurobehavioral  function and that could account for  some
differences in results among studies is  the rather precipitous increase in  lead
exposure observed in most of the prospective studies during the first 2-3  years
of life.   Sizeable rises  in  postnatal  blood lead levels  were  noted in the
Cincinnati, Cleveland, and Port Pirie  studies,  but not  in the Boston study.

3/27/89                              59

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
        TABLE 13.   STRONGEST RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN BLOOD LEAD MEASURES
             AT SPECIFIED TIMES AND LATER NEUROBEHAVIORAL OUTCOMES
                      AS DETECTED BY PROSPECTIVE STUDIES
Study
      Time/Type
of Blood Lead Measure
     Outcome
Boston
(Bellinger
et al.,  1987a,b)
Cincinnati
(Dietrich
et al.,  1987b,
1988; Bhatta-
charya et al.,
1988, 1989)

Cleveland
(Ernhart
et al.,  1986,
1987a; Wolf et
al., 1985;
Morrow-Tlucak
& Ernhart, 1987)

Pt.  Pi He
(Wigg et al. ,
1988; McMichael
et al.,  1988)

Sydney
(Cooney et al.,
1989b)
Delivery/cord


2 yr

Prenatal (X:  16 wk)/maternal

10 day

2 yr


Delivery/cord


Deli very/maternal

Delivery/cord


6 month

6, 24, 36 month and
 integrated postnatal

Cumulative (prior plus
 current)
6- ,  12-, 18-, and
 24-month MDI

5-yr McCarthy

3- and 6-month MDI

12-month MDI

6-yr postural sway


12-month MDI (via 30-day
 G-R soft signs)

6-month MDI, PDI, KID

2-yr language acquisition
 (2 measures)

2-yr MDI

4-yr McCarthy
36 to 48 month change in
 cognitive development (p
 0.14, two-tailed)
None  of  the first  three studies showed  a significant association  between

prenatal  lead  exposure  indicators  and 2-year MDI scores,  but the Boston study

did.  While  analyses from  the  Boston study also  indicated that continuing

"high" lead  exposure (>10 ug/dl) contributed to a  persistence  of neurobehav-

ioral deficits  at  later ages, a high postnatal  blood lead in the Boston cohort

was  not  as  high as the  average  for  any of the  other prospective studies at age

2 years.   Thus,  the relationship between lead exposure at a critical stage of

early development  and subsequent neurobehavioral  function could be obscured by

differential lead  exposure  (most often,  but not always,  increased exposure) in

the  intervening period.
3/27/89
                  60

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     By this  line  of reasoning,  one would perhaps expect that increased post-
natal PbB measures  should  then show a  significant relationship  to  later neuro-
behavioral outcomes.  This was in  fact the  case  in the  Port  Pirie  study, where
sample size was more clearly large enough to afford relatively good statistical
power.  (This  also  points  up,  as discussed above,  why  it is not possible at
present to discriminate between prenatal and postnatal  lead exposure in stating
a blood lead level  of concern for developmental neurobehavioral  effects.)
     It should also  be  kept  in mind that  scores  on tests  such as the  Bayley  or
McCarthy Scales are only indicators or reflections of neurobehavioral  function.
Such variables may  be valid  and  reliable  mesures,  but they do not  fully repre-
sent all  aspects of a  child's cognitive, social,  and emotional development.
Thus, considerable  caution must  be  exercised  in  drawing conclusions from find-
ings of "no  effect."   Not  only are  there many facets  to a  child's develop-
ment that need to  be assessed,  but  these facets may interact in complex ways
that may  be  quite  difficult to detect or evaluate.   For example, a  child's
emotional  and  social adaptation  (including  such notions as "self-esteem") may
be  influenced  in  subtle as  well as  obvious  ways by his or her cognitive
abilities.
     Such complexities make  it difficult  to presume that  a failure to detect a
continuing association between an  indicator of prenatal  lead exposure and, for
example, scores on  the  McCarthy  Scales at 5 years is  evidence of no permanent
effect.   It is well known that the nervous system is capable  of adapting to and
even compensating for various insults during early development.   But it is also
true that the  full  realization of  developmental  potential can very much depend
on events during critical  stages of ontogeny.   A  parallel may  be drawn with
impaired language acquisition in  children whose hearing  has been  affected by
chronic otitis media (e.g.,  Kavanagh,  1986).   Although  otitis media itself may
be transient and  fully reversible,  it secondary effects  on language development
in young children may be much longer lived.   As noted by Jenkins (1986, p.  216),
"With a fluctuating  hearing  loss,  such children may receive inconsistent and
inadequate information,  or may have to devote so much attention to the decoding
process itself that there is  little  capacity  left over for  higher-order
cognitive operations.  If  children  are, in  fact, doing  something different or
expending more resources on  lower  levels of speech perception, we may see
deficits in other processes at a  later period."
3/27/89                              61

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
     It is also  important  to  note the convergence of animal findings, particu-
larly those showing  impairments  in  higher-level  behavioral processes such as
discrimination reversal learning in  primates  as  well  as rodents at blood lead
levels below 20  ug/dl.  A  number of such studies,  conducted in at least three
independent laboratories,   provide evidence  free  of the  socioeconomic and other
complex variables  that sometimes  complicate the interpretation of  the  human
literature.  These studies were  summarized  and reviewed  in  Air  Quality
Criteria for Lead (U.S. EPA, 1986a).
     A remaining issue  is  whether  the effects discussed here  are  large  enough
to constitute a  significant risk  to public health.  Valuable  information has
been provided  by various  prospective  studies on  the  magnitude  of neuro-
behavioral deficits  relative  to increments  in  blood lead  levels.   Despite
different approaches to treating their  data,  three of the prospective  studies
provide results  suggesting that  Bayley  MDI scores decline  by  2-8 points for
approximately every 10-ug/dl  increase  in blood lead level.  The  Boston  study
found 4-8  point  differences in  6-  to 24-month MDI scores between high  (mean:
14.6 ug/dl) and  low  (mean: 1.8 ug/dl) cord blood lead groups.   The Cincinnati
study showed as  much  as an 8.4-point decline  in boys  6-month MDI scores for
every 10-ug/dl increment in maternal  blood lead.  Also,  the Port Pirie  study
demonstrated a 1.6-point decrease  in the 24-month MDI  per  10  ug/dl of  blood
lead at 6 months, and a ~3.5-point  decrease in GCI  scores for  every 10 ug/dl  in
average postnatal  blood lead.   As  noted by Davis and Svendsgaard (1987),  an
overall 4-point  downward shift  in  a normal distribution of scores such  as  the
MDI or GCI would result in 50 percent more children  scoring below 80 on these
exams (cf. Needleman, 1983; McMichael et al.,  1988).
3/27/89                              62

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
IV.  REFERENCES


Baghurst, P.  A.;  Robertson, E. F. ; McMichael,  A.  J.;  Vimpani, G.  V.;  Wigg,
     N. R.;  Roberts,  R.  R.  (1987) The Port Pirie cohort study:  lead effects on
     pregnancy  outcome  and early  childhood development.  Neurotoxicology
     8: 395-402.

Bellinger, D.  C. ;  Needleman,  H.  L.;  Leviton, A.;  Waternaux,  C.;  Rabinowitz,
     M. B.;  Nichols, M. L. (1984a) Early sensory-motor development and prenatal
     exposure to lead. Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 6: 387-402.

Bellinger, D. ;  Needleman,  H.  L. ;  Bromfield, R. ;  Mintz,  M.  (1984b)  A followup
     study of  the  academic attainment and classroom behavior of children with
     elevated dentine lead levels. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 6: 207-223.

Bellinger, D.; Leviton, A.; Waternaux, C.; Needleman, H.; Rabinowitz, M. (1985)
     A  longitudinal  study of  the developmental  toxicity  of  low-level lead
     exposure  in the  prenatal  and early postnatal periods. In: Lekkas, T. D.,
     ed.  International conference:  heavy metals in the environment; September;
     Athens, Greece,  v.  1.  Edinburgh, United Kingdom:  CEP  Consultants, Ltd.;
     pp. 32-34.

Bellinger, D. ;  Leviton,  A.;  Needleman, H.  L.;  Waternaux,  C.;  Rabinowitz, M.
     (1986a) Low-level lead  exposure  and infant development in the  first year.
     Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 8:  151-161.

Bellinger, D.;  Leviton,  A.;  Rabinowitz, M.; Needleman,  H.;  Waternaux,  C.
     (1986b) Correlates of low-level  lead exposure in urban children at 2 years
     of age.  Pediatrics 77: 826-833.

Bellinger, D. ;  Leviton,  A.;  Waternaux,  C. ; Needleman,  H.; Rabinowitz,  M.
     (1987a) Longitudinal  analyses  of prenatal  and postnatal  lead exposure and
     early cognitive development.  N.  Engl. J. Med. 316: 1037-1043.

Bellinger,  D. ;  Sloman,  J.;  Leviton, A.;  Waternaux,   C. ;  Needleman,  H. ;
     Rabinowitz, M.  (1987b) Low-level  lead exposure and  child  development:
     assessment at  age 5 of a cohort  followed  from birth.  In:  Lindberg, S.  E.;
     Hutchinson, T.  C.,  eds.  International conference: heavy metals  in  the
     environment, v.  1;  September;  New Orleans, LA.  Edinburgh,  United Kingdom:
     CEP Consultants, Ltd.; pp. 49-53.

Bellinger, D.; Leviton, A.; Waternaux, C.; Needleman, H.; Rabinowitz, M. (1988)
     Low-level  lead  exposure,  social  class,   and  infant  development.
     Neurotoxicol.  Teratol. 10: in press.

Bellinger, D. ;  Leviton,  A.;  Waternaux,  C. ; Needleman,  H. ; Rabinowitz,  M.
     (1989a) Low-level lead exposure  and early development in socioeconomically
     advantaged urban  infants.  In:  Smith, M. A.;  Grant,  L.  D.;  Sors, A. I.,
     eds. Lead  exposure  and  child development:  an  international  assessment
     [international workshop  on effects of  lead  exposure  on  neurobehavioural
     development];  September 1986; Edinburgh, United Kingdom.  Lancaster, United
     Kingdom:  Kluwer Academic Publishers; pp. 345-356.
3/27/89                              63

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Bellinger, D. ; Leviton, A.;  Sloman,  J.  (1989b) Antecedents  and  correlates  of
     improved cognitive performance  in children exposed  in  utero to  low levels
     of lead. Presented at:  Conference  on advances in lead research: implica-
     tions for  environmental  research.  Research Triangle Park,  NC:  National
     Institute of Environmental  Health Sciences;  January.

Benetou-Marantidou, A.; Nakou,  S.; Micheloyannis, J.  (1988) Neurobehavioral
     estimation of  children  with  life-long  increased lead  exposure.  Arch.
     Environ. Health 43:  392-395.

Bhattacharya, A.; Shukla,  R.; Bornschein,  R.; Dietrich, K.;  Kopke, J. E. (1988)
     Postural disequilibrium  quantification in  children with chronic  lead
     exposure:  a pilot study.  Neurotoxicology 9:  327-340.

Bhattacharya, A.; Shukla,  R. ; Bornschein, R.;  Dietrich, K. ;  Keith,  R.  (1989)
     Lead effects  on balance and equilibrium. Presented at:  Conference on
     advances in  lead  research:  implications  for environmental  research.
     Research Triangle  Park,  NC:  National Institute  of  Environmental  Health
     Services; January.

Bornschein,   R. L.;  Grote,  J.;  Mitchell,  T., Succop.  P.  A.; Dietrich, K. N.;
     Krafft,  K.  M.;  Hammond, P. B.  (1989) Effects  of  prenatal  lead exposure on
     infant  size at  birth.  In:  Smith, M.   A.;  Grant,  L.  D.;  Sors, A.  I.,  eds.
     Lead exposure and child development:  an international  assessment [interna-
     tional   workshop on effects  of lead exposure on neurobehavioural develop-
     ment];   September  1986;  Edinburgh,   United  Kingdom. Lancaster,  United
     Kingdom: Kluwer Academic Publishers;  pp. 307-319.

Boscolo,  P.;  Carmignani, M.  (1988) Neurohumoral  blood pressure  regulation  in
     lead exposure.  In:  Victery,  W.,  ed. Symposium  on  lead-blood pressure
     relationships; April   1987; Chapel Hill, NC.  EHP  Environ.  Health Perspect.
     78: 101-106.

Chai,  S.;  Webb,  R.  C.  (1988) Effects of lead on vascular  reactivity.  In:
     Victery, W. ,  ed.  Symposium  on  lead-blood pressure  relationships;  April
     1987; Chapel Hill,  NC. EHP Environ.  Health Perspect. 78: 85-89.

Cluydts,  R.  J.  G. ; Steenhout,  A.  (1989)  A regression analysis  study of the
     Brussels lead  and  IQ  data.   In: Smith,  M. A.; Grant,  L. D. ; Sors,  A.  I.,
     eds. Lead  exposure and child development:  an international assessment
     [international workshop on effects  of lead exposure on neurobehavioural
     development]; September 1986; Edinburgh, United Kingdom. Lancaster, United
     Kingdom: Kluwer Academic Publishers;  pp, 251-254.

Cohen, J. (1977)  Statistical  power analysis for  the  behavioral  sciences.  New
     York, NY: Academic Press.

Cooney,  G.  H.;  McBride, W.;  Bell, A.; Carter,  C. (1989a)  Neurobehavioural
     consequences of prenatal low level exposures  to  lead.  J. Neurol. Teratol.:
     accepted for publication.
3/27/89                              64

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Cooney, G. H.; Bell, A.; McBride, W. ; Carter, C. (1989b)  Low  level exposures  to
     lead: the Sydney  lead study at  four  years.  J.  Dev. Med. Child  Neurol.:
     accepted for publication.

Cooper, W. C.  (1988) Deaths from chronic  renal  disease  in U. S. battery  and
     lead  production workers.   In:  Victery, W., ed.  Symposium on lead-blood
     pressure relationships; April  1987; Chapel Hill, NC. EHP Environ.  Health
     Perspect. 78: 61-63.

Davis,  J.  M.; Svendsgaard, D.  J.  (1987) Lead and child development. Nature
     (London) 329: 297-300.

de Kort,  W.  L.  A. M.; Zwennis, W. C. M. (1988) Blood  lead and blood  pressure:
     some  implications  for the situation  in the Netherlands.  In: Victery, W.,
     ed.  Symposium  on lead-blood pressure relationships; April  1987; Chapel
     Hill, NC. EHP Environ. Health Perspect. 78: 67-70.

de Kort,  W.  L. ;  Verschoor, M.  A. ;  Wibowo, A.  A. ; van Hemmen, J. J.  (1987)
     Occupational exposure  to  lead  and blood pressure: a study in 105 workers.
     Am. J.  Ind.  Med. 11: 145-156.

Dietrich, K.  N.; Krafft, K. M.; Bier, M.;  Succop, P. A.;  Berger,  0.;  Bornschein,
     R.  L.   (1986)  Early effects of fetal  lead  exposure:  neurobehavioral
     findings at 6 months.  Int. J. Biosoc.  Res. 8: 151-168.

Dietrich,  K.   N.;  Krafft, K. M.;  Shukla,  R.;  Bornschein, R.  L. ;  Succop, P. A.
     (1987a)  The neurobehavioral effects of early lead exposure.  In:  Schroeder,
     S.  R.,  ed.  Toxic  substances  and mental   retardation:  neurobehavioral
     toxicology and  teratology.  Washington, DC:  American Association on Mental
     Deficiency;  pp.  71-95. (Begab,  M.  J., ed. Monographs  of the American
     Association on Mental  Deficiency: no. 8).

Dietrich,  K.   N. ;  Krafft, K. M.; Bornschein, R.  L.;  Hammond,  P. B.; Berger, 0.;
     Succop,  P. A. ;  Bier,  M.   (1987b)  Low-level  fetal  lead exposure effect on
     neurobehavioral development in early  infancy. Pediatrics  80: 721-730.

Dietrich,  K.  N. ;  Krafft, K. M.;  Shukla,  R.; Bornschein, R.  L.;  Berger, 0.;
     Succop,   P.  A.; Hammond,  P.  B.  (1988) Developmental patterns  of neuro-
     behavioral deficit  and recovery in infancy: response  to fetal and early
     postnatal lead exposure [unpublished  material]. Cincinnati,  OH:  University
     of Cincinnati, College of Medicine.

Dietrich,  K. N.;  Krafft,  K.  M.; Bier,  M. ; Berger,  0.; Succop,  P.  A.;
     Bornschein,  R.  L.  (1989a)  Neurobehavioural  effects  of foetal lead
     exposure: the  first year of life. In: Smith, M.  A.  ; Grant, L.  D.; Sors,
     A. I.,  eds.  Lead  exposure and  child development:  an international assess-
     ment  [international workshop  on effects  of  lead  exposure  on  neuro-
     behavioural   development]; September  1986; Edinburgh,  United Kingdom.
     Lancaster, United Kingdom: Kluwer Academic Publishers; pp. 320-331.
3/27/89                              65

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Dietrich, K. N. ;  Succop.  P.  A.; Bornschein, R. L.; Krafft, K. M. ; Berger, 0.;
     Hammond, P.  B. ;  Buncher,  C.  R.  (1989b) Lead exposure and neurobehavioral
     development in later infancy. Presented at: Conference on advances in lead
     research;  implications for environmental research. Research Triangle Park,
     NC:  National Institute of Environmental Health Services; January.

E.  I. Du  Pont  de Nemours. (1986) Du  Pont comments  on the February 1986 draft
     document.  Wilmington, DE:  E.  I.  Du Pont de Nemours & Co., Inc. Available
     for inspection at:  U.  S.  Environmental Protection Agency, Central Docket
     Section, Washington, DC; docket no. ECAO-CD-81-2 IIA.E.C.3.6.

Elwood,  P.  C.;  Davey-Smith,  G. ; Oldham, P. D.; Toothill, C. (1988a) Two Welsh
     surveys of  blood lead and blood pressure. In: Victery, W., ed. Symposium
     on  lead-blood  pressure  relationships; April 1987;  Chapel  Hill,  NC.  EHP
     Environ. Health Perspect.  78: 119-121.

Elwood,  P.  C.;  Yarnell,  J. W.; Oldham,  P.  D.; Catford, J. C.;  Nutbeam,  D.;
     Davey-Smith, G.;  Toothill,  C.  (1988b) Blood pressure  and blood lead in
     surveys in Wales. Am. J. Epidemiol. 127: 942-945.

Ernhart, C.  B.;  Morrow-Tlucak,  M. (1989) Low-level lead exposure and intelli-
     gence  in the  early  preschool years.  In:  Smith, M.  A.; Grant,  L.  D.;  Sors,
     A.   I., eds.  Lead  exposure  and  child development:  an  international
     assessment  [international  workshop  on  effects  of  lead exposure on
     neurobehavioural   development]; September 1986; Edinburgh, United Kingdom.
     Lancaster, United Kingdom: Kluwer Academic Publishers; pp. 469-474.

Ernhart, C.  B.;  Wolf, A. W.  ; Kennard, M. J.; Filipovich, H. F.; Sokol, R. J. ;
     Erhard, P.  (1985a)  Intrauterine  lead  exposure and the status of the neo-
     nate.  In:  Lekkas, T. D., ed. International conference: heavy metals in the
     environment; September; Athens,  Greece,  v. 1. Edinburgh, United Kingdom:
     CEP Consultants,   Ltd.; pp. 35-37.

Ernhart, C.  B. ;  Wolf, A. W.  ; Kennard,  M.  J.; Erhard, P.; Filipovich,  H. F.;
     Sokol,  R.  J.  (1986)  Intrauterine exposure to  low  levels of lead: the
     status of the neonate. Arch. Environ.  Health 41: 287-291.

Ernhart, C. B.; Morrow-Tlucak, M.; Marler,  M.  R.; Wolf, A. W.  (1987a) Low level
     lead exposure in  the prenatal and early preschool periods: early preschool
     development. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 9: 259-270.

Ernhart,  C.  B.;  Morrow-Tlucak,  M.  (1987b)  Low level lead  exposure  in the
     prenatal and  early  preschool years as related to intelligence just prior
     to  school  entry. In:  Lindberg,  S. E.;  Hutchinson,  T.  C., eds. Inter-
     national  conference:  heavy metals  in  the environment,  v. 1; September;
     New  Orleans,  LA.  Edinburgh, United Kingdom:  CEP  Consultants, Ltd.;
     pp. 150-152.

Ernhart,  C.  B.;  Wolf,  A. W.;  Linn,  P. L. ;  Sokol,  R.  J. ; Kennard, M. J. ;
     Filipovich,  H.   F.  (1985b)  Alcohol-related birth  defects:  syndromal
     anomalies,  intrauterine growth  retardation,  and  neonatal  behavioral
     assessment. Alcohol.  Clin. Exp. Res.  9:  447-453.
3/27/89
66

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Ernhart, C. B.;  Morrow-THucak,  M.;  Wolf, A. W. (1988) Low level lead exposure
     and intelligence in the preschool years. Sci. Total Environ. 71: 453-459.

Fanning, D. (1988) A mortality study of lead workers, 1926-1985. Arch. Environ.
     Health 43(3): 247-251.

Faust,  D.;  Brown,  J.  (1987) Moderately elevated blood lead levels: effects on
     neuropsychologic functioning in children. Pediatrics 80: 623-629.

Fergusson,   J.  E.;  Purchase, N.  G.   (1987)  The analysis and levels of lead in
     human teeth: a review. Environ. Pollut. Ser. A 46: 11-44.

Fergusson,   D.  M.;  Fergusson,  J.  E.; Norwood,  L.  J.;  Kinzett, N. G.  (1988a) A
     longitudinal study  of dentine  lead levels,  intelligence,  school  perfor-
     mance  and behaviour:  part  I. Dentine  lead and cognitive ability.  J.  Child
     Psychol.  Psychiatry Allied Discip. 29: 781-792.

Fergusson,   D.  M. ;  Fergusson,  J.  E.  ; Norwood,  L.  J. ;  Kinzett, N. G.  (1988b) A
     longitudinal study  of dentine  lead levels,  intelligence,  school  perfor-
     mance and behaviour: part II.  Dentine lead and cognitive ability. J. Child
     Psychol.  Psychiatry Allied Discip. 29: 793-809.

Fergusson,   D.  M.;  Fergusson,  J.  E.; Norwood,  L.  J.;  Kinzett, N. G.  (1988c) A
     longitudinal study  of dentine  lead levels,  intelligence,  school  perfor-
     mance and behaviour: part III.  Dentine lead levels and attention/activity.
     J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 29: 811-824.

Frohlich,  E.  D.   (1983)  Mechanisms  contributing to high blood  pressure.  In:
     McCarron, D.  A.; Kotchen,  T.  A.,  eds.  Nutrition and  blood  pressure:
     current status of dietary factors and hypertension, a symposium; September
     1982;  Arlington, VA. Ann. Intern. Med. 98 (5, pt. 2): 709-714.

Fulton, M.; Raab,  G.; Thomson,  G.;  Laxen,  D.;  Hunter, R.;  Hepburn,  W.  (1987)
     Influence of blood lead on the ability and  attainment of children in
     Edinburgh. Lancet (8544): 1221-1226.

Gartside,  P.  S.  (1985)  Pb hypertension: NHANES  II  -  statistical overview and
     evaluation.   In:  Goldwater,  L. ; Wysocki, L. M.; Volpe, R. A., eds. Edited
     proceedings: lead environmental  health - the current issues;  May;  Durham,
     NC. Durham,  NC: Duke University; pp. 27-32.

Gartside, P. S. (1988) The relationship of blood lead  levels  and blood pressure
     in NHANES II:  additional  calculations.   In: Victery, W., ed. Symposium on
     lead-blood  pressure  relationships; April  1987;  Chapel  Hill,  NC.  EHP
     Environ.  Health Perspect. 78:  31-34.

Grandjean,   P.; Hollnagel,  H.;  Hedegaard, L. ; Christensen,  J.  M. ;  Larsen, S.
     (1989)  Blood  lead-blood pressure  relationships  alcohol   intake  and
     hemoglobin as confounders.  Am.  J. Epidemiol.: in press.
3/27/89                              67

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Grant, L. D.;  Davis,  J.  M.  (1989)  Effects  of  low-level  lead  exposure  on paedi-
     atric  neurobehavioural  and  development:  current  findings and  future
     directions.  In:  Smith,  M.  A.; Grant,  L.  D.;  Sors,  A. I., eds. Lead expo-
     sure and  child development:  an  international  assessment [international
     workshop  on  effects of lead  exposure  on  neurobehavioural  development];
     September 1986;  Edinburgh, United  Kingdom.  Lancaster,   United  Kingdom:
     Kluwer Academic Publishers; pp. 49-115.

Graziano, J.;  Popovac,  D. ;  Murphy, M. ;  Mehmeti,  A.;  Kline,  J. ; Ahmedi,  G.;
     Shrout, P.; Zvicer, Z.; Wasserman,  G.; Gashi,  E.;  Stein,  Z.;  Rajovic,  B.;
     Belmont,  L. ;  Colakovic,  B. ;   Bozovic,  R.;  Haxhiu,  R. ; Radovic,  L.;
     Vlaskovic, R. ;  Nenezic, D.;  Loiacono,  N. (1989a)  Environmental  lead,
     reproduction and infant development. In:  Smith, M.  A.; Grant, L.  D.; Sors,
     A. I., eds.Lead exposure and child development: an international assessment
     [international workshop on effects  of lead exposure  on  neurobehavioural
     development]; September 1986;  Edinburgh,  United Kingdom. Lancaster, United
     Kingdom: Kluwer Academic Publishers; pp.  379-386.

Graziano, J.;  Popovac,  D.;  Factor-Litvak,  P.; Shrout,  P.; Kline,  J.; Murphy,
     M. J.; Zhao,  Y.; Mehmeti,  A.;  Ahmedi,  G.; Rajovic,  B.; Zvicer, Z.; Nenezic,
     D. U. ;  Loiacono, N.  J. ; Stein, Z. (1989b) The influence of environmental
     lead exposure  on human pregnancy outcome.  Presented at:  Conference  on
     advances  in  lead  research:   implications  for environmental  research.
     Research  Triangle  Park, NC:  National  Institute  of Environmental  Health
     Sciences; January.

Hansen, 0. N.; Trillingsgaard,  A.;  Beese, I.;  Lyngbye, T.; Grandjean, P. (1989)
     Neuropsychological  profile of children in relation to dentine lead level
     and socioeconomic group. In: Smith, M. A.; Grant, L. D.; Sors, A.  I., eds.
     Lead exposure and child development: an international assessment [interna-
     tional workshop  on  effects of lead exposure on neurobehavioural develop-
     ment];  September 1986;  Edinburgh,  United  Kingdom.  Lancaster,  United
     Kingdom: Kluwer Academic Publishers; pp.  240-250.

Harlan, W.  R.  (1988)  The relationship of  blood  lead  levels  to blood pressure
     in  the U. S. population.   In:  Victery, W.,  ed.  Symposium  on  lead-blood
     pressure  relationships; April  1987; Chapel  Hill, NC. EHP Environ. Health
     Perspect. 78: 9-13.

Harlan, W. R.; Landis, J. R.; Schmouder, R. L.; Goldstein, N. G.; Harlan, L. C.
     (1985) Blood  lead  and  blood pressure  relationship  in the adolescent and
     adult U S population.  JAMA J.  Am. Med. Assoc. 253:  530-534.

Harvey,  P.  G.; Hamlin,  M.  W. ;  Kumar, R. ;  Delves,  H. T.  (1984) Blood  lead,
     behaviour and  intelligence test performance in  preschool  children.  Sci.
     Total  Environ. 40:  45-60.
3/27/89                              68

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Hatzakis, A.;  Kokkevi,  A.;  Katsouyanni, K.; Maravelias,  K. ;  Salaminios, F. ;
     Kalandidi, A.;  Koutselinis, A.;  Stefanis,  K. ; Trichopoulos,  D.  (1987)
     Psychometric  intelligence  and  attentional  performance  deficits  in
     lead-exposed  children.  In:  Lindberg,  S.  E.; Hutchinson,  T.   C.,  eds.
     International  conference:  heavy metals  in the  environment,  v.  1;
     September; New Orleans,  LA.  Edinburgh, United Kingdom: CEP  Consultants,
     Ltd.; pp. 204-209.

Hatzakis, A.;  Kokkevi,  A.;  Maravelias, C. ;  Katsouyanni,  K. ;  Salaminios, F.;
     Kalandidi, A.;  Koutselim's, A.;  Stefanis,  C.; Trichopoulos,  D.  (1989)
     Psychometric  intelligence deficits in lead-exposed  children.  In:  Smith,
     M. A.; Grant,  L.  D. ;  Sors, A.  I.,  eds.  Lead  exposure and child develop-
     ment: an  international  assessment [international  workshop on  effects  of
     lead exposure on neurobehavioural development]; September 1986; Edinburgh,
     United Kingdom.  Lancaster,  United  Kingdom:  Kluwer Academic Publishers;
     pp. 211-223.

Jenkins, J. J.  (1986)  Cognitive development in children with recurrent otitis
     media:  where  do we stand? In:  Kavanagh,  J.  F.,  ed.  Otitis media and child
     development:  [proceedings of a conference; September, 1985; Bethesda, MD].
     Parkton,  MD:  York Press: pp. 211-221.  (Communicating by language series).

Kaplan, N. M.  (1983)  Hypertension:  prevalence,  risks,  and effect of therapy.
     In: McCarron, D. A. ;  Kotchen,  T. A. ,  eds.  Nutrition and blood pressure
     control:  current status  of  dietary factors  and  hypertension,  a symposium;
     September 1982; Arlington, VA.  Ann. Intern.  Med. 98  (5, pt. 2): 705-709.

Kavanagh, J.  F., ed.  (1986) Otitis media and child development: [proceedings  of
     a  conference;  September,  1985; Bethesda, MD].  Parkton,  MD:  York Press.
     (Communicating by language series).

Kirkby, H. ; Gyntelberg,  F.  (1985)  Blood pressure and other cardiovascular risk
     factors  of  long-term  exposure  to lead.  Scand.  J. Work Environ.  Health
     11: 15-19.

Kopp, S. J. ;  Barron,  J.  T.; Tow, J.  P.  (1988) Cardiovascular  actions of lead
     and relationship to hypertension: a review.  In:  Victery, W., ed. Symposium
     on  lead-blood  pressure relationships; April 1987;  Chapel  Hill, NC. EHP
     Environ.  Health Perspect. 78:  91-99.

Kromhout, D.  (1988) Blood  lead and  coronary  heart  disease risk among elderly
     men  in Zutphen,  The Netherlands. In:  Victery, W.,  ed.  Symposium on
     lead-blood pressure  relationships; April 1987;  Chapel  Hill,  NC.  EHP
     Environ.  Health Perspect. 78:  43-46.

Landis, J. R.;  Flegal,  K.  M. (1988) A generalized Mantel-Haenszel  analysis of
     the regression of  blood pressure on blood lead using NHANES II data. In:
     Victery,  W.,  ed.  Symposium on  lead-blood pressure relationships; April
     1987; Chapel  Hill,  NC.  EHP Environ. Health Perspect. 78: 35-41.

Lansdown, R.;   Yule, W.;  Urbanowicz, M. A.; Hunter, J.  (1986) The relationship
     between blood lead  concentrations,  intelligence,  attainment and behaviour
     in a  school  population:  the  second  London study.  Int.  Arch. Occup.
     Environ.  Health 57: 225-235.


3/27/89                              69

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Marler, M. R.;  Ernhart,  C.  B. (1987) Early childhood lead burden and physical
     growth.  In: Lindberg, S.  E.; Hutchinson, T. C. , eds. International confer-
     ence: heavy metals  in  the environment, v. 1; September; New Orleans, LA.
     Edinburgh, United Kingdom: CEP Consultants, Ltd.; pp. 148-149.

McBride, W. G. ;  Carter,  C.  J.; Bratel, J. R. ; Cooney, G.; Bell, A. (1989) The
     Sydney study of  health effects of lead  in urban children.  In:  Smith,  M.
     A.; Grant,  L.  D.;  Sors,  A.  I., eds.  Lead exposure and child development:
     an  international  assessment [international workshop on  effects of lead
     exposure  on neurobehavioural  development];  September 1986;  Edinburgh,
     United Kingdom.  Lancaster,  United Kingdom:  Kluwer Academic Publishers;
     pp. 255-259.

McCarron,  D.  A.; Kotchen,  T.  A., eds. (1983)  Nutrition and blood pressure
     control:  current status  of  dietary factors and hypertension,  a symposium;
     September 1982; Arlington, VA. Ann. Intern. Med. 98  (5, pt. 2).

McGee,  D.; Gordon,  T.  (1976)  The  results  of the Framingham Study applied to
     four  other  U.  S.-based epidemiologic studies of  coronary  heart disease:
     section 31. In:  Kannel,  W.  B.; Gordon, T., eds. The Framingham Study:  an
     epidemiological investigation  of cardiovascular disease.  Washington, DC:
     U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, National Institutes
     of  Health;  DHEW publication  no.  (NIH)  76-1083.  Available from:  GPO,
     Washington, DC: S/N 017-040-00396-0.

McMichael, A. J.; Vimpani,  G. V.;  Robertson,  E.  F.; Baghurst,  P. A.; Clark,
     P. D.  (1986)   The  Port  Pirie  cohort study:  maternal  blood  lead  and
     pregnancy outcome.  J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 40:  18-25.

McMichael, A.  J.;  Baghurst, P. A.;  Wigg,  N. R.; Vimpani, G. V.;  Robertson,
     E. F.; Roberts,  R.  J.  (1988) Port  Pirie cohort  study:  environmental
     exposure to lead and children's abilities  at  the age of four years. N.
     Engl. J.  Med.  319:  468-475.

Moore,  M.  R.; Goldberg,  A.;  Pocock,  S.  J.; Meredith, A.; Stewart,  I.  M.;
     Macanespie, H.;  Lees,  R.; Low, A. (1982a)  Some studies of maternal and
     infant lead exposure in Glasgow. Scott. Med. J. 27:  113-122.

Moore,  M.  R. ;  Goldberg,  A.; Bushnell,  I.   W.  R. ; Day, R. ; Fyfe, W. M.  (1982b)
     A  prospective  study of  the neurological  effects  of lead  in children.
     Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 4: 739-743.

Moore,  M.  R.;  Bushnell,  I.  W. R.; Goldberg, Sir A.  (1989) A prospective study
     of the results of  changes in  environmental  lead exposure in children in
     Glasgow.  In: Smith,  M.  A.;  Grant, L. D.;  Sors, A.  I., eds. Lead exposure
     and child development: an international assessment  [international workshop
     on effects  of  lead exposure on  neurobehavioural  development];  September
     1986;  Edinburgh,  United Kingdom. Lancaster,  United  Kingdom:  Kluwer
     Academic Publishers; pp.  371-378.
3/27/89                              70

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Moreau, T.; Hannaert,  P.;  Orssaud, G. ; Hue!, G. ; Garay, R. P.; Claude, J. R.;
     Juguet, B.;  Festy,  B.;  Lellouch, J. (1988)  Influence of membrane sodium
     transport  upon the  relation between blood lead  and  blood pressure  in a
     general male population.  In:  Victery,  W., ed.  Symposium  on  lead-blood
     pressure  relationships; April  1987; Chapel Hill, NC. EHP Environ. Health
     Perspect.  78: 47-51.

Morrow-Tlucak,   M. ;  Ernhart,  C.  B.  (1987) The relationship of low level  lead
     exposure  and language  development in the pre-school  years.  In  Lindberg,
     S. E.; Hutchinson,  T.  C. ,  eds.  International conference: heavy metals in
     the  environment,  v. 1;  September;  New Orleans, LA.  Edinburgh, United
     Kingdom:  CEP Consultants, Ltd.; pp.  57-59.

Needleman, H.   L.  (1983)  Low level  lead exposure and neuropsychological perfor-
     mance. In:  Rutter,  M.;  Russell  Jones,  R. , eds.  Lead versus health. New
     York, NY:  John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.; pp. 229-248.

Needleman, H.   L.; Gunnoe,  C.;  Leviton,  A.;  Reed,  R.; Peresie, H. ;  Maher, C. ;
     Barrett,   P.  (1979)  Deficits in psychologic and  classroom performance of
     children with elevated dentine lead levels. N. Engl.  J. Med. 300: 689-695.

Neri,  L.  C. ;   Hewitt,  D. ;  Orser,  B.  (1988)  Blood lead and  blood  pressure:
     analysis   of cross-sectional  and longitudinal  data  from Canada. In:
     Victery,   W. , ed.  Symposium on  lead-blood  pressure relationships; April
     1987; Chapel Hill, NC. EHP Environ.  Health Perspect.  78:  123-126.

Parkinson, D.   K.; Hodgson,  M.  J. ; Bromet, E. J. ;  Dew, M. A.; Connell, M. M.
     (1987) Occupational lead  exposure and blood  pressure.  Br.  J.  Ind.  Med.
     44: 744-748.

Pirkle, J.  L.;  Schwartz, J.;  Landis,  J.  R.;  Harlan,  W.  R.  (1985) The relation-
     ship  between blood  lead levels and blood pressure and its cardiovascular
     risk  implications. Am. J.  Epidemiol. 121:  246-258.

Pocock, S. J.;  Shaper, A.  G. ; Ashby,  D. ;  Delves,  T.  ; Whitehead, T.  P. (1984)
     Blood lead  concentration,  blood pressure,  and renal  function.  Br. Med.  J.
     289:  872-874.

Pocock, S. J.;  Shaper, A. G.; Ashby, D.;  Delves, T. (1985) Blood lead  and blood
     pressure   in  middle-aged men.  In:  Lekkas, T.  D.,  ed.  International confer-
     ence: heavy  metals  in the environment,  v.  1;  September; Athens, Greece.
     Edinburgh, United Kingdom: CEP Consultants, Ltd.; pp. 303-305.

Pocock, S.  J. ; Ashby,  D. ;  Smith,  M.  A.  (1987)  Lead  exposure  and  children's
     intellectual performance.  Int. J. Epidemiol. 16: 57-67.

Pocock, S. J. ;  Shaper, A.  G. ; Ashby, D.; Delves, H.  T.; Clayton, B.  E. (1988)
     The  relationship  between  blood lead, blood  pressure, stroke,  and heart
     attacks in middle-aged british  men.  In:  Victery, W., ed.  Symposium on
     lead-blood  pressure relationships;  April  1987;  Chapel  Hill,  NC.  EHP
     Environ.  Health Perspect.  78:  23-30.
3/27/89                              71

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Pooling Project Research Group.  (1978)  Relationship of blood pressure,  serum
     cholesterol,  smoking  habit,  relative weight  and ECG  abnormalities to
     incidence of major coronary  events:  final  report of the pooling project.
     J.  Chronic Dis.  31:  201-306.

Raab, G.  M. ;  Fulton, M. ;  Thomson,  G.  0.  B. ; Laxen,  D.  P.  H. ; Hunter,  R. ;
     Hepburn,  W. (1989) Blood  lead  and other influences on mental abilities -
     results from the Edinburgh  lead study.  In: Smith, M.  A. ;  Grant, L. D. ;
     Sors, A.   I,, eds.  Lead exposure and child  development:  an  international
     assessment [international  workshop  on  effects  of  lead exposure  on
     neurobehavioural development];   September 1986;  Edinburgh, United Kingdom.
     Lancaster, United Kingdom:  Kluwer Academic  Publishers;  pp.  183-200.

Richards, W. N. ; Moore,  M.  R.  (1984) Lead hazard controlled in Scottish water
     systems.  Am.  Water Works Assoc. J.  76:  60-67.

Rothenberg, S. J. ; Schnaas,  L. ;  Mendez, C.   J.  N. ;  Hidalgo,  H. (1989) Effects
     of lead on neurobehavioural  development in  the first thirty  days of life.
     In: Smith, M. A.; Grant,  L.  D.; Sors, A. I.,  eds.  Lead exposure and child
     development:  an international   assessment   [international  workshop  on
     effects of lead exposure on neurobehavioural development]; September 1986;
     Edinburgh, United  Kingdom.  Lancaster,   United Kingdom:  Kluwer  Academic
     Publishers; pp.  387-395.

Schroeder, S.   R. ; Hawk,  B.  (1987)  Psycho-social factors,  lead  exposure, and
     IQ. In: Schroeder, S.  R. ,  ed.   Toxic substances  and  mental  retardation:
     neurobehavioral   toxicology  and  teratology.   Washington,  DC: American
     Association of  Mental  Deficiency; pp.  97-137.  (Begab,  M.  J. , ed.
     Monographs of the American Association  on Mental Deficiency:  no. 8).

Schwartz,  J.  (1985a) Evidence  for  a blood   lead-blood pressure  relationship
     [memorandum to  the Clean  Air Science Advisory Committee]. Washington,  DC:
     U.  S.  Environmental Protection Agency,  Office of Policy Analysis. Avail-
     able  for  inspection at:  U.  S.   Environmental  Protection Agency, Central
     Docket Section,  Washington, DC; docket  no.  ECAO-CD-81-2 IIA.F.60.

Schwartz,  J.  (1985b) Response  to Richard Royall's  questions on  the blood
     lead-blood pressure relationships  in NHANES II  [memorandum  to  Dr.  David
     Weil]. Washington,  DC:  U.  S.   Environmental  Protection Agency, Office
     of Policy  Analysis.  Available  for inspection  at:  U.  S. Environmental
     Protection  Agency,  Central  Docket  Section,   Washington,  DC;  docket
     no. ECAO-CD-81-2 IIA.C.5.

Schwartz,  J.   (1986a)  NHANES  II  blood pressure  analysis  [memorandum  to
     Dr. Lester Grant].  Washington,  DC: U.  S. Environmental  Protection Agency,
     Office of  Policy Analysis.  Available for inspection at:  U.  S.   Environ-
     mental Protection Agency,  Central  Docket Section, Washington, DC;  docket
     no. ECAO-CD-81-2 IIA.C.10.

Schwartz,  J.  (1986b) Blood  lead and blood  pressure (again) [memorandum to
     Dr. Lester Grant].  Washington,  DC: U.  S. Environmental Protection Agency,
     Office of  Policy Analysis.  Available for  inspection at:  U.  S.   Environ-
     mental Protection Agency,  Central  Docket Section, Washington, DC;  docket
     no. ECAO-CD-81-2 IIA.C.ll.


3/27/89                              72

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Schwartz, J.  (1988) The  relationship  between blood lead and  blood pressure
     in  the  NHANES II survey.  In:  Victery,  W.,  ed. Symposium on lead-blood
     pressure relationships;  April  1987;  Chapel  Hill, NC. EHP Environ. Health
     Perspect. 78: 15-22.

Schwartz, J.  (1989)  Lead, blood pressure, and cardiovascular  disease in men
     and  women.   In:  Foweler,  B.,  ed.  Lead in  bone conference;  March;
     Baltimore, MD. EHP Environ. Health Perspect.: accepted for publication.

Schwartz, J.;  Otto, D.  (1987)  Blood  lead,  hearing thresholds,  and neuro-
     behavioral  development  in  children  and youth.  Arch.  Environ.  Health
     42: 153-160.

Scragg,  R.  K.  R.; McMichael, A.  J. ;  Clark,  P.  D.  (1977)  Pollution and  sex
     ratio of births [letter]. Med.  J.  Aust.  2:  68.

Selevan, S.  G. ;  Landrigan,  P. J. ; Stern,  F.  B.;  Jones,  J.  H.  (1988) Lead and
     hypertension  in a  mortality study of lead smelter  workers.  In: Victery,
     W., ed. Symposium on lead-blood pressure relationships; April  1987;  Chapel
     Hill, NC. EHP Environ.  Health Perspect.  78:  65-66.

Shaper, A.  G. ;  Pocock,  S. J.; Walker,  M. ;  Cohen,  N.  M. ;  Wale,  C.  J.; Thompson,
     A. G.  (1981) British regional  heart  study:  cardiovascular risk factors in
     middle-aged men in 24 towns. Br.  Med. J. 283: 179-186.

Sharp,  D.  S.; Becker,  C.  E.  ;  Smith,  A.  H.  (1987)  Chronic low-level lead
     exposure:  its  role  in the pathogenesis of  hypertension.  Med. Toxicol.
     2: 210-232.

Sharp,  D. S. ;  Osterloh,  J.; Becker, C. E.; Bernard, B.; Smith, A.  H. ; Fisher,
     J. M.;  Syme,  S.  L.;  Holman,  B.  L.; Johnston, T.  (1988) Blood pressure and
     blood lead concentration in bus drivers. In:  Victery, W., ed.  Symposium on
     lead-blood  pressure relationships;  April  1987;  Chapel  Hill,  NC.  EHP
     Environ. Health Perspect. 78: 131-137.

Shucard, D.  W. ;  Shucard, J.  L. ; Thomas,  D. G.  (1988a) Neurophysiological
     studies of  human  cognitive development in premature infants:  an approach
     to the study of maturational brain processes. Neurotoxicology  9: 299-316.

Shucard, J.  L.;  Shucard,  D.  W.  ; Patterson,  R.; Guthrie, R. (1988b)  Prenatal
     lead exposure  and  its  potential  significance for developmental  disabili-
     ties:  a  preliminary  study  of  umbilical  cord  blood  lead  levels.
     Neurotoxicology 9: 317-326.

Shukla,  R.;  Bornschein,   R.  L.; Dietrich, K.  N. ; Mitchell, T.;  Grote,  J. ;
     Berger,  0.;  Hammond,  P.  B.; Succop,  P.  A.   (1987) Effects of fetal
     and  early  postnatal lead  exposure  on child's growth  in  stature -  the
     Cincinnati  lead  study.   In:  Lindberg,  S. E.;  Hutchinson, T.  C.,  eds.
     International  conference:   heavy  metals  in  the  environment,  v.  1;
     September; New Orleans,  LA.  Edinburgh,  United Kingdom: CEP  Consultants,
     Ltd.; pp. 210-212.
3/27/89                              73

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:   DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Staessen, J.;  Bruaux,  P.;  Claeys-Thoreau,  F.; DePlaen,  P.;  Ducoffre,  G.;
     Lauwerys,  R. ;  Roels,  H.; Rondia, D. ;  Sartor,  F. ;  Amery, A.  (1988)  The
     relationship between blood pressure and environmental exposure to lead and
     cadmium in  Belguim.  In:  Victery,  W.,  ed.  Symposium on  lead-blood pressure
     relationships; April 1987;  Chapel Hill,  NC.  EHP  Environ.  Health  Perspect.
     78: 127-129.

Tanner, J. M. (1981) Catch-up growth in man. Br. Med.  Bull.  37: 233-238.

Tyroler, H.   A.  (1988)  Epidemiology  of  hypertension  as a public health problem:
     an  overview as background  for evaluation of blood  lead-blood pressure
     relationship.  In:  Victery,  W.,  ed.  Symposium  on   lead-blood  pressure
     relationships; April 1987;  Chapel Hill,  NC.  EHP  Environ.  Health  Perspect.
     78: 3-7.

U. S.  Environmental  Protection  Agency.  (1977) Air quality  criteria for lead.
     Research Triangle Park,  NC:  Health  Effects Research Laboratory,  Criteria
     and  Special  Studies Office; EPA  report  no.  EPA-600/8-77-017. Available
     from: NTIS, Springfield, VA; PB-280411.

U.  S.   Environmental Protection Agency.   (1986a)  Air quality  criteria for
     lead.  Research Triangle Park,  NC:  Office of  Health and Environmental
     Assessment,  Environmental  Criteria  and  Assessment  Office;  EPA  report
     no. EPA-600/8-83/028aF-dF.   4v.  Available  from:  NTIS,  Springfield,  VA;
     PB87-142378.

U. S.  Environmental  Protection  Agency.  (1986b) Lead effects on cardiovascular
     function,  early  development,  and stature: an  addendum to U.S.  EPA Air
     Quality  Criteria  for  Lead  (1986).  In: Air  quality criteria for lead,
     v.  1.  Research Triangle  Park, NC:  Office of  Health and Environmental
     Assessment,  Environmental  Criteria and  Assessment  Office;  pp.  A1-A67;
     EPA  report no. EPA-600/8-83/028aF.   Available  from: NTIS,  Springfield,
     VA; PB87-142378.

Vander,  A.  J.  (1988) Chronic effects of lead on the renin-angiotensin system.
     In: Victery, W., ed. Symposium on lead-blood pressure relationships; April
     1987; Chapel Hill, NC.  EHP Environ.  Health Perspect. 78: 77-83.

Victery, W.,  ed.  (1988a) Symposium  on  lead-blood  pressure relationships; April
     1987; Chapel Hill, NC.  EHP Environ.  Health Perspect. 78.

Victery,  W.  (1988b) Evidence for effects  of chronic lead exposure on blood
     pressure  in experimental  animals:  an overview. In: Victery, W.,  ed.
     Symposium  on  lead-blood pressure  relationships; April 1987;  Chapel  Hill,
     NC. EHP Environ. Health Perspect.  78:  71-76.

Victery,  W.; Tyroler,  H. A.; Volpe,  R.;  Grant,  L.  D.  (1988)  Summary of
     discussion  sessions: symposium on lead-blood pressure relationships. In:
     Victery,  W.,  ed.  Symposium  on lead-blood pressure  relationships;  April
     1987; Chapel Hill, NC.  EHP Environ.  Health Perspect. 78:  139-155.
3/27/89                              74

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Vimpani, G.  V.;  Wigg, N. R.;  Robertson,  E-  F.;  McMichael, A.  J.;  Baghurst,
     P. A.;  Roberts,  R.  J.  (1985)  The Port Pirie  cohort  study:  blood lead
     concentration  and  childhood  developmental  assessment.  In:  Goldwater,
     L. J.  ;  Wysocki,  L.  M.;  Volpe,  R. A.,  eds. Edited  proceedings:  Lead
     environmental health -  the current issues; May;  Durham,  NC.  Durham, NC:
     Duke University; pp. 139-146.

Vimpani, G. ;  Baghurst,  P.;  McMichael,  A.  J.;  Robertson, E.;  Wigg,  N.;  Roberts,
     R. (1989) The  effects  of cumulative  lead exposure on pregnancy outcome
     and childhood  development during  the first four years.  Presented  at:
     Conference on  advances  in lead research: implications  for environmental
     research. Research  Triangle  Park,  NC: National Institute of Environmental
     Health Sciences; January.

Vivoli, G.;  Bergomi,  M. ; Borella, P.;  Fantuzzi, G. ;  Simoni,  L.;  Catelli, D.;
     Sturloni, N.; Cavazzuti, G. B.; Montorsi, R.; Tampieri, A.; Tartoni, P. L.
     (1989)  Evaluation of different  biological  indicators of  lead exposure
     related  to  neuropsychological  effects  in  children.  In:  Smith,  M.   A.;
     Grant,  L. D.;  Sors, A.  I., eds.  Lead exposure and child development:  an
     international  assessment  [international  workshop  on  effects of  lead
     exposure  on neurobehavioural development];  September 1986;  Edinburgh,
     United  Kingdom.  Lancaster, United Kingdom:  Kluwer Academic Publishers;
     pp. 224-239.

Ward,  N. I.;  Watson,  R.  ; Bryce-Smith,  D.  (1987)  Placental  element levels  in
     relation to  fetal  development  for obstetrically "normal" births: a  study
     of 37  elements.  Evidence for effects of cadmium,  lead and zinc on fetal
     growth,  and  for  smoking  as  a  source of cadmium.  Int.  J.  Biosoc.  Res.
     9(1):  63-81.

Wedeen, R.  P. (1988)  Bone   lead, hypertension,  and  lead  nephropathy.  In:
     Victery, W. ,  ed. Symposium on  lead-blood  pressure relationships; April
     1987; Chapel Hill,  NC.   EHP Environ. Health Perspect. 78: 57-60.

Weiler, E.;  Khali1-Manesh,  F. ; Gonick, H. (1988) Effects  of lead  and natri-
     uretic  hormone  on kinetics  of  sodium-potassium-activated  adenosine
     triphosphatase:  possible  relevance to hypertension. In: Victery, W.,  ed.
     Symposium on lead-blood pressure relationships; April 1987; Chapel  Hill,
     NC. EHP Environ. Health Perspect.  78: 113-115.

Weiss,  S.   T.;  Munoz,  A.; Stein, A.;  Sparrow,  D.;  Speizer, F.  E.  (1988)  The
     relationship of  blood  lead to  systolic blood  pressure  in a longitudinal
     study of policemen.  In: Victery,  W.,  ed.  Symposium on lead-blood pressure
     relationships;  April 1987;  Chapel  Hill,  NC.  EHP Environ.  Health Perspect.
     78: 53-56.

Wigg,  N.  R.; Vimpani, G. V.;  McMichael,  A.  J.;  Baghurst, P.  A.;  Robertson,
     E. F.; Roberts, R. J.  (1988) Port  Pirie cohort study: childhood blood  lead
     and neuropsychological  development at age two years. J. Epidemic!. Commun.
     Health 42:  213-219.

Wilson, R.  S. (1986)  Growth and development  of human twins.  In: Tanner,
     J. M.;  Falkner,  F. , eds.  Human growth, v.  3. New  York, NY: Plenum Press;
     pp. 197-211.


3/27/89                              75

-------
                   PRELIMINARY DRAFT:  DO NOT QUOTE OR CITE
Winneke, G. ;  Beginn,  U. ;  Ewert, T. ; Havestadt,  C. ;  Kraemer, U. ;  Krause,  C.;
     Thron, H. L. ; Wagner, H. M. (1985a) Comparing the effects  of  perinatal  and
     later childhood lead exposure  on neuropsychological outcome.  Environ.  Re:;.
     38: 155-167.

Winneke, G. ;  Brockhaus,  A.;  Collet, W.; Kraemer, U. ; Krause, C. ; Thron, H. L. ;
     Wagner,  H.  M. (1985b)  Predictive value  of different markers of  lead-
     exposure  for neuropsychological  performance.   In:  Lekkas, T.  D., ed.
     International conference:  heavy metals  in  the  environment;  September;
     Athens, Greece,  v.  1.  Edinburgh, United  Kingdom:  CEP Consultants, Ltd.;
     pp. 44-47.

Winneke, G.;  Collet,  W.; Kraemer,  U.;  Brockhaus, A.; Ewert,  T.;  Krause, C.
     (1989a)  Follow-up  studies  in  lead-exposed  children.  In:  Smith, M. A.;
     Grant, L.  D.; Sors,  A.   I., eds.  Lead exposure and child development: an
     international  assessment  [international  workshop  on effects  of  lead
     exposure  on  neurobehavioural  development]; September  1986;  Edinburgh,
     United Kingdom.  Lancaster, United Kingdom: Kluwer Academic  Publishers;
     pp. 260-270.

Winneke, G.; Lilienthal, H.;  Ewert, T.  (1989b)  Effects of  lead  on  electrophysi-
     ological   and  performance measures: animal  and  human  data. Presented at:
     Conference  on advances  in lead research:  implications for environmental
     research.  Research Triangle Park,  NC: National Institute of Environmental
     Health Sciences; January.

Wolf, A. W.; Ernhart, C. B.;  White, C.  S.  (1985) Intrauterine  lead exposure and
     early  development.  In:  Lekkas, T.  D., ed.  International conference:  heavy
     metals in the environment; September; Athens,  Greece, v.  2.  Edinburgh,
     United Kingdom: CEP Consultants, Ltd.; pp.  153-155.

Wolf,  A.  W.;  Lozoff,  B. ; Jimenez,  E.  (1987)  Lead and  infant development  in
     a  developing  country.   In:  Lindberg,  S.  E.; Hutchinson,  T.  C. ,  eds.
     International  conference:  heavy  metals  in  the environment,   v.  1;
     September;  New Orleans,  LA. Edinburgh, United  Kingdom:  CEP Consultants,
     Ltd.;  pp. 165-167.
                 * US GOVERNMENT PRINTING OFBCf. 1989- 648- 010.-00 00 3
3/27/89                               76

-------