-------
Table 6-9. (cent.)
:- Set of Candidate Standards
:UnC0fpeted
Roar Dust-
Lead
Loading
.teg/ft*)
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
Window
SOT Dust-
Lead
Loading
{figlfft
125
125
125
125
125
125
125
Max.,
Aim.OfV
•Damaged
Paint on a
; Tested
Surface
{%>'•'
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
9 Units
At Or
Above
; At Least
;;One
Stsnd&rd
/Total*
Units*
166/189
167/189
169/189
169/189
170/189
179/189
185/189
- Performance C
Sensitivity ,
#(%> of Units
wfflhlBUf'V
Ctiildr6nwjnat
Are At or Above
At Least One
r'StflHuoiu „-
42/45 (93.3%)
43/45 (95.6%)
44/45 (97.8%)
44/45 (97.8%)
44/45 (97.8%)
45/45 (100%)
45/45 (100%)
Specificity
#(%) of Units
with No EBL
Chadren That Are
At or Above No
St&ndsfd^
20/144(13.9%)
20/144(13.9%)
19/144(13.2%)
19/144(13.2%)
18/144(12.5%)
10/144(6.9%)
4/144 (2.8%)
». ••mjUiu-Si-rijte
Ml CtblOl H4MA
PPV
#(%) of Units
At or Above At;
•• Least One ' "~
Standard That-
HaveffiL:
Chadren5
42/166(25.3%)
43/167(25.7%)
44/169(26.0%)
44/169(26.0%)
44/170(25.9%)
45/179(25.1%)
45/185(24.3%)
NPV
# (%) of Units At
'- '» or -Above Ma
Standard Tnat Do
_/ Not Have EBL
Children'
20/23 (87.0%)
20/22 (90.9%)
19/20(95.0%)
19/20(95.0%)
18/19(94.7%)
10/10(100%)
4/4 (100%)
1 In the Rochester study, each measurement of lead in paint had the amount of damaged paint specified as * 15%" (poor condition) of the tested surface, with no indication of total damaged surface area.
1 Total number of units having available data that could be compared to all specified candidate standards.
9 Cell entries areinumber of homes at or above at least one standard that have EBL children)/ number of homes containing EBL children),
followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
4 Cell entries are (number of homes not at or above at least one standard that do not have EBL children)/(total number of homes not
containing EBL children), followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
* Cell entries are (number of homes at or above at least one standard that have EBL chik)ren)/(total number of homes at or above at least
one standard), followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
' Cell entries are (number of homes not at or above at least one standard that do not have EBL children)/(total number of homes not at or
above any standard), followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
264
-------
Table 6-10. Results of Performance Characteristics Analysis Performed on Data for
Housing Units in the Rochester Lead-in-Dust Study, for Specified Sets of
Candidate Standards for Dust-Lead Loadings and Observed Amount of
Damaged Lead-Based Paint on a Tested Surface
EBL = elevated blood-lead level (:>10//g/dU; LBP=Lead-Based Paint
Set of Candidate Standards
Uncarpeted
Floor Dust-
Lead
Loading
tt*g/f*l
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
100
50
40
25
20
10
Window
Sin Dust-
Lead ~
Loading
frig/ft*).
-
-
-
-
-
- •
-
250
250
250
250
250
250
250
125
125
125
125
125
125
125
-
-
-
-
-
-
Max.
Aim. Of
Damaged
LBPona
Tested:
Surface"'
:<%)'
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
_
-
-
-
• -
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
# Units
At Or.
Above
At Least
One
$t&nd8rd'
/Total*
: Units2
9/197
19/197
31/197
58/197
84/197
150/197
180/197
71/189
75/189
80/189
93/189
106/189
150/189
176/189
116/189
118/189
122/189
128/189
134/189
159/189
180/189
84/197
88/197
94/197
104/197
115/197
162/197
Performance Characteristics
Sensitivity
#<%) of Units
with EBL
CrfldrenThat
Aro At or Above
At Least One
' * Standard^
5/47 (10.6%)
9/47(19.1%)
16/47(34.0%)
26/47 (55.3%)
31/47 (66.0%)
44/47 (93.6%)
45/47 (95.7%)
25/45 (55.6%)
27/45 (60.0%)
30/45 (66.7%)
34/45 (75.6%)
36/45 (80.0%)
44/45 (97.8%)
44/45 (97.8%)
35/45 (77.8%)
36/45 (80.0%)
38/45 (84.4%)
39/45 (86.7%)
40/45 (88.9%)
45/45 (100%)
45/45 (100%)
27/47 (57.4%)
28/47 (59.6%)
31/47(66.0%)
33/47 (70.2%)
36/47 (76.6%)
44/47 (93.6%)
Soeeificitv
# (%) of Units
with No &L
Children That Are
At or Above No
Standard4
146/150(97.3%)
140/150(93.3%)
135/150(90.0%)
118/150(78.7%)
97/150(64.7%)
44/150(29.3%)
15/150(10.0%)
98/144(68.1%)
96/144166.7%)
94/144(65.3%)
85/144(59.0%)
74/144(51.4%)
38/144 (26.4%)
12/144(8.3%)
63/144 (43.8%)
62/144(43.1%)
60/144(41.7%)
55/144(38.2%)
50/144(34.7%)
30/144 (20.8%)
9/144 (6.3%)
93/150(62.0%)
90/150(60.0%)
87/150(58.0%)
79/150(52.7%)
71/150(47.3%)
32/150(21.3%)
PPV
* (%) of Units,,
At or Above At
Least One-
Standard; That ;
Have EBL
Chadren*
5/9 (55.6%)
9/1 9 (47.4%)
16/31 (51.6%)
26/58 (44.8%)
31/84(36.9%)
44/150(29.3%)
45/180(25.0%)
25/71 (35.2%)
27/75 (36.0%)
30/80 (37.5%)
34/93 (36.6%)
36/106 (34.0%)
44/150(29.3%)
44/176(25.0%)
35/116(30.2%)
36/118(30.5%)
38/122(31.1%)
39/128(30.5%)
40/134(29.9%)
45/159 (28.3%)
45/180(25.0%)
27/84(32.1%)
28/88(31.8%)
31/94(33.0%)
33/104(31.7%)
36/115(31.3%)
44/162(27.2%)
NPV
#{%} of Units At
• or Above No
Standard That Do
Not Have EBL
CtiikifSfii *«,
146/188(77.7%)
140/178 (78.7%)
135/166 (81.3%)
118/139(84.9%)
97/113(85.8%)
44/47 (93.6%)
15/17(88.2%)
98/118(83.1%)
96/114(84.2%)
94/109(86.2%)
85/96 (88.5%)
74/83 (89.2%)
38/39 (97.4%)
12/13 (92.3%)
63/73 (86.3%)
62/71 (87.3%)
60/67 (89.6%)
55/61 (90.2%)
50/55 (90.9%)
30/30(100%)
9/9 (100%)
93/113(82.3%)
90/109 (82.6%)
87/103 (84.5%)
79/93 (84,9%)
71/82(86.6%)
32/35(91.4%)
265
-------
Table 6-10. (cont.)
Set of Candidate Standards _ -
Uncarpeted
Floor Dust-
Lead
Loading
-torn1)
5
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
100
50
40
25
20
10
5
Window
SiD Dust-
Lead
Lending
(ra/frt
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
250
250
250
250
250
250
250
125
125
125
125
125
125
125
250
250
250
250
250
250
250
Max.
Ami. Of :
Damaged
LBPona
Tested.
'Surface
(%)'
>15%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>S%
>5%
>5%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>15%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
# Units
At Or
Above
At Least
One
.St&ndara
/Total*
Units2
183/197
146/197
147/197
149/197
150/197
155/197
181/197
189/197
107/189
109/189
114/189
119/189
126/189
160/189
178/189
135/189
137/189
141/189
144/189
147/189
167/189
181/189
147/189
148/189
150/189
151/189
156/189
175/189
182/189
Sensitivity
# (%) of Units
withEBL
Children That:
Are At or Above
At Least One
Standard: ~
45/47 (95.7%)
39/47 (83.0%)
40/47 (85.1%)
41/47 (87.2%)
42/47 (89.4%)
44/47 (93.6%)
47/47 (100%)
47/47 (100%)
32/45(71.1%)
33/45 (73.3%)
36/45 (80.0%)
37/45 (82.2%)
39/45 (86.7%)
44/45 (97.8%)
44/45 (97.8%)
37/45 (82.2%)
38/45 (84.4%)
40/45 (88.9%)
41/45(91.1%)
42/45 (93.3%)
45/45 (100%)
45/45 (100%)
38/45 (84.4%)
39/45 (86.7%)
40/45 (88.9%)
41/45(91.1%)
43/45 (95.6%)
45/45 (100%)
45/45 (100%)
. . ^ . ^
Specificity
# (%) of Units
with No SL
Chfldren That Are
At or Above No
Standard?
12/150(8.0%)
43/1 50 (28.7%)
43/1 50 (28.7%)
42/150(28.0%)
42/150(28.0%)
39/150(26.0%)
16/150(10.7%)
8/150(5.3%)
69/144 (47.9%)
68/144(47.2%)
66/144(45.8%)
62/144(43.1%)
57/144(39.6%)
28/144(19.4%)
10/144(6.9%)
46/144(31.9%)
45/144(31.3%)
43/144(29.9%)
41/144 (28.5%)
39/144(27.1%)
22/144(15.3%)
8/144(5.6%)
35/144 (24.3%)
35/144(24.3%)
34/144(23.6%)
34/144(23.6%)
31/144(21.5%)
14/144(9.7%)
7/144(4.9%)
P£V
# (%) of Units
At or Above At
Least One
Standard That
HaveEBL
dhadren5
45/183(24.6%)
39/146 (26.7%)
40/147 (27.2%)
41/149 (27.5%)
42/150(28.0%)
44/155(28.4%)
47/181 (26.0%)
47/189 (24.9%)
32/107.(29.9%)
33/109(30.3%)
36/114(31.6%)
37/119(31.1%)
39/126(31.0%)
44/160(27.5%)
44/178(24.7%)
37/135(27.4%)
38/137(27.7%)
40/141 (28.4%)
41/144 (28.5%)
42/147 (28.6%)
45/167(26.9%)
45/181 (24.9%)
38/147 (25.9%)
39/148(26.4%)
40/150(26.7%)
41/151 (27.2%)
43/156(27.6%)
45/175(25.7%)
45/182(24.7%)
• <•
NPV
# (%) of Units At
or Above No
Standard That Do
Not HaveEBL
Children*
12/14(85.7%)
43/51 (84.3%)
43/50 (86.0%)
42/48 (87.5%)
42/47 (89.4%)
39/42 (92.9%)
16/16(100%)
8/8 (100%)
69/82(84.1%)
68/80 (85.0%)
66/75 (88.0%)
62/70 (88.6%)
57/63 (90.5%)
28/29 (96.6%)
10/11 (90.9%)
46/54 (85.2%)
45/52 (86.5%)
43/48 (89.6%)
41/45(91.1%)
39/42 (92.9%)
22/22 (100%)
8/8(100%)
35/42 (83.3%)
35/41 (85.4%)
34/39 (87.2%)
34/38 (89.5%)
31/33 (93.9%)
14/14(100%)
7/7(100%)
266
-------
Table 6-10. (cont.)
•••: Set of Candidate Standards
»""*•- f--£il "X.
ROOT Dust-
Lead "
Loading
100
50
4O
25
20
10
5
Window
Sffl Dust-
Lead
Loading
(i*g/ft*>*
12S
125
125
125
125
125
125
Max.:;
Amt. Of
Damaged
LBPona
tested-
Surface
(%)'
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
>5%
# Units
At Or
Above
At Least
One
Standard
/Total #
Units -
156/189
157/189
159/189
160/189
163/189
176/189
183/189
V " . , * '••<-.. -Performance C
f "* - • -t ' ~
Sensitivity
# (%) of Units
with EBL •'
Children That
Are At or Above:
At Least One
~ Standard^.
40/45 (88.9%)
41/45(91.1%!
42/45 (93.3%)
43/45 (95.6%)
44/45 (97.8%)
45/45 (100%)
45/45 (100%)
Specificity
#(%) of Units
with No EBL
Children That Are
At or Above Mo
Standard* :
28/144(19.4%)
28/144(19.4%)
27/144(18.8%)
27/144(18.8%)
25/144(17.4%)
13/144(9.0%)
6/144 (4.2%)
haracteristics
Bfv
-V.
# (%) of Units
At or Above At
Least One "
Standard That
Have EBL
Children5
40/156(25.6%)
41/157(26.1%)
42/159(26.4%)
43/160(26.9%)
44/163(27.0%)
45/176(25.6%)
45/183(24.6%)
-
- NPV
#(%)ofUnhsAt
or Above No
Standard That Do
Not Have EBL
Children*
28/33 (84.8%)
28/32 (87.5%)
27/30 (90.0%)
27/29(93.1%)
25/26 (96.2%)
13/13(100%)
6/6 (100%)
11n the Rochester study, each measurement of lead in paint had the amount of damaged paint specified as "<5%" (good condition), *5-
15%' (fair condition), or "> 15%' (poor condition) of the tested surface, with no indication of total damaged surface area.
2 Total number of units having available data that could be compared to all specified candidate standards.
* Cell entries arelnumber of homes at or above at least one standard that have EBL children)/ number of homes containing EBL children),
followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
* Cell entries are (number of homes not at or above at least one standard that do not have EBL children)/(total number of homes not
containing EBL children), followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
* Cell entries are (number of homes at or above at least one standard that have EBL chtldren)/(total number of homes at or above at least
one standard), followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
' Call entries are (number of homes not at or above at least one standard that do not have EBL children)/(total number of homes not at or
above any standard), followed by the corresponding percentage (in parentheses).
267
-------
6.2 INVESTIGATING INCIDENCE OF ELEVATED BLOOD-LEAD
CONCENTRATION IN HOUSING UNITS MEETING ALL
EXAMPLE OPTIONS FOR STANDARDS
An alternative to the performance characteristics analysis approach (Section 6.1) to
evaluating a set of candidate standards is to use statistical modeling techniques to predict a
distribution of blood-lead concentration as a function of environmental-lead levels found in
homes which do not exceed any of the candidate standards, then estimate the percentage of
children residing in these homes that are expected to have elevated blood-lead levels (i.e., at or
above 10 ug/dL). It is desired to select a set of candidate standards so that the likelihood of
children with elevated blood-lead concentration residing in homes that do not exceed any of the
candidate standards would be very low. This section presents a modeling approach to estimate
this likelihood, using the alternative Rochester multimedia model presented in Section 4.2 of this
report ("Model A" in Table 4-1), and applies this approach to data from the Rochester study.
Recall from Section 4.2 that the reason for developing the alternative Rochester
multimedia model was to have the risk estimates from model-based analyses be more comparable
to the results of the performance characteristics analysis presented in the §403 proposed rule
(Section 6.1.3) and the results of the follow-up performance characteristics analyses (Section
6.1.4). In particular, both the performance characteristics analysis and the model-based approach
involving the alternative Rochester multimedia model use the following types of data as input
when characterizing risk:
» household average (wipe) dust-lead loading from uncarpeted floors
• household average (wipe) dust-lead loading from window sills
* yard-wide average soil-lead concentration
* the larger of the following two percentages: % of interior tested surfaces that
contain deteriorated lead-based paint (LBP), and % of exterior tested surfaces that
contain deteriorated LBP
In the model-based analysis approach presented below, the candidate standards were used to
identify a subset of homes in the Rochester study that were below all of the candidate standards,
calculate the average (across homes) of the above three measures of lead levels in dust and soil,
and fit the multimedia model to these average lead levels in order to predict a distribution of
blood-lead concentrations for children residing in these homes. For simplicity, this analysis
assumes that the homes do not contain deteriorated lead-based paint. Because the slope estimate
for the paint variable in the alternative Rochester multimedia model is nearly zero (Table 4-1 of
Section 4.2), making the assumption that no deteriorated lead-based paint exists in these homes
should have a very minor impact on the resulting risk estimates.
6.2.1 The Model-Based Approach
This model-based approach had the following four steps:
268
-------
1. For a given set of candidate standards for floor dust-lead loading, window sill
dust-lead loading, and soil-lead concentration, identify those homes in the
Rochester study that exceed none of the candidate standards in this set.
2. For each of the following three household measures, calculate the average across
the homes identified in step #1: the household average floor dust-lead loadings,
household average window sill dust-lead loading and for yard-wide average soil-
lead concentration. These three averages are assumed to represent lead levels in
housing represented by the Rochester study homes in step #1 (i.e., homes not
exceeding any of the candidate dust and soil standards).
3. Use the three averages calculated in step #2 as input to the alternative Rochester
multimedia model from Section 4.2 (assuming no .deteriorated lead-based paint
exists in the units).
4. Assume that log-transformed blood-lead concentration for children residing in the
homes identified in step #1 is normally distributed with mean equal to the
predicted log-transformed blood-lead concentration that is output from the model
fitting in step #3, and standard deviation equal to ln(1.6). (Recall that this
assumption on variability was made throughout the §403 risk analysis.) Using
normal distribution theory, determine the percentage of children represented by
this blood-lead distribution that have log-transformed blood-lead concentration or
above log(10), or equivalently, that have blood-lead concentration at or above 10
ug/dL.
6.2.2 Examples of Applying the Model-Based Approach
To illustrate how the approach in Section 6.2.1 is applied to data from the Rochester
study, the following combinations of candidate dust-lead and soil-lead standards are considered:
• (uncarpeted) floor dust-lead loading: either 40 or 50 (ig/ft2
• window sill dust-lead loading: 250 ug/ft2
• yard-wide soil-lead concentration: 400 fig/g.
When the candidate floor dust-lead loading standard is 40 ug/ft2. then the performance
characteristics analyses documented in Table 6-8 of Section 6.1 (i.e., the row of Table 6-8
corresponding to these three candidate standards) indicates that 39 of the 184 Rochester study
homes having measurements for dust-lead, soil-lead, and deteriorated lead-based paint do not
exceed any of the three candidate standards. Across these 39 homes, the following averages were
calculated from the Rochester study data:
household average (uncarpeted) floor dust-lead loading: 12.7 ug/ft2
household average window sill dust-lead loading: 87.0 ug/ft2
269
-------
* yard-wide average soil-lead concentration: 125.3 ug/g.
When fitting the alternative Rochester multimedia model to these three averages (assuming no
deteriorated lead-based paint), the model predicts a geometric mean blood-lead concentration of
4.68 ug/dL. If the standard deviation of log-transformed data is assumed to be 1.6 and normal
distribution theory is applied as described above, then the estimated percentage of children with
blood-lead concentration at or above 10 ug/dL in homes that do not exceed any of the candidate
standards is 5.30%. This matches closely with the estimate of 5.1 %, or 2 of these 39 homes in
the Rochester study dataset, which the performance characteristics analysis (Table 6-8) indicated
contained children with elevated blood-lead concentrations.
If the candidate floor dust-lead loading standard is increased to 50 ug/ft2. then the number
of Rochester study homes having measurements for dust-lead, soil-lead, and deteriorated lead-
based paint and that do not exceed any of the three candidate standards increases by one home, to
40 total homes. Across these 40 homes, the following averages were calculated from the
Rochester study data:
* household average (uncarpeted) floor dust-lead loading: 13.4 ug/ft2
• household average window sill dust-lead loading: 85.6 fig/ft2
« yard-wide average soil-lead concentration: 122.2 ug/g.
The predicted geometric mean blood-lead concentration under these assumed dust-lead and soil-
lead levels (assuming no deteriorated lead-based paint) is 4.69 ug/dL. and the estimated
percentage of children with blood-lead concentration at or above 10 ug/dL is 5.34%. This is a
very slight increase from the estimate generated under the candidate floor dust-lead loading
standard of 40 ug/ft2. The performance characteristics analysis (Table 6-8) indicated that under
these candidate standards, 7.5% of homes not exceeding any of the standards (i.e., 3 of these 40
homes in the Rochester study dataset) contained children with elevated blood-lead
concentrations.
While these examples illustrate the estimation process, they also-show that the number of
homes in the given dataset whose lead levels fall below all specified candidate standards can be
quite small, especially when at least one of the candidate standards is set at the low end of the
distribution of lead levels (i.e., most homes have data that fall above the candidate standard).
Therefore, as the set of candidate standards becomes more stringent, and as the size of the sample
from which the environmental-lead data originate becomes smaller as a result, the variability
associated with the estimated risk increases. Furthermore, as the set of candidate standards
becomes less stringent (i.e., as the standards increase), the group of homes not exceeding any of
the candidate standards is more likely to remain the same, and as a result, the estimated risk
eventually reaches a plateau. This occurs in the above examples, as increasing the candidate
floor dust-lead loading standard from 40 to 50 ug/ft2 does little, if any, to increase the estimated
risk beyond 5.3% under this approach and under the given set of data, assuming the candidate
standards for the other media (window sill dust, soil) remain fixed.
270
-------
The Rochester study data were used in this analysis as the multimedia model was fitted
based on the Rochester data. If data from other studies were used instead, it would be necessary
to verify that the model parameter estimates adequately reflect the underlying variability in these
data in the same manner that they reflect variability in the Rochester study data.
While the approach presented in this section is relatively easy to implement, it could be
modified even further in an attempt to achieve more accurate risk estimates. Such a modification
could reduce the level of simplicity associated with applying the approach. For example, rather
than calculate average environmental-lead levels across all homes and fit the model once to these
averages, a simulation approach could be applied in an attempt to more accurately represent the
entire distribution of environmental-lead levels in these homes and the resulting blood-lead
distribution associated with exposure across the entire distribution of environmental-lead levels.
6.3 REVIEW OF PUBLISHED INFORMATION ON POST-INTERVENTION
DUST-LEAD LOADINGS
This section summarizes published information on lead loadings (amount of lead per unit
surface area) in dust samples collected by wipe techniques, as reported by earlier lead
intervention studies. This information is used to evaluate assumptions made on post-intervention
dust-lead loadings (40 ug/ft2 for floors, 100 ug/ft2 for window sills) within the §403 risk analysis.
Details to supplement the summaries in this section are presented in Appendix H.
The following seven studies have been identified in which some type of paint or dust
intervention was performed, dust samples were collected using wipes or some other technique
(e.g., BRM vacuum) whose results could be converted to wipe-equivalent dust-lead loadings, and
post-intervention dust-lead loadings on floors and/or window sills were reported (references for
these studies are included in Appendix H):
• Baltimore Experimental Paint Abatement Studies
• Baltimore Follow-up Paint Abatement Study
• Baltimore Repair & Maintenance (R&M) Study
• Boston Interim Dust Intervention Study
• HUD Grantees Evaluation (data available through September 1997)
• Denver Comprehensive Abatement Performance (CAP) Study
• Jersey City Children's Lead Exposure and Reduction (CLEAR) Study
These studies employed a variety of intervention strategies, including single or repeated dust
cleanings and interim control or complete abatement of lead-based paint. Dust-lead loadings
were measured at varying intervals following intervention. Post-intervention dust-lead loadings
were summarized for 19 groups of housing units across these seven studies. These study groups
are defined hi Appendix H.
For both floors and window sills, geometric mean and median dust-lead loadings were
observed below the post-intervention assumptions established in the §403 risk analysis in a
271
-------
majority of the study groups. However, this does not preclude results for individual housing
units from being above the assumed levels. Furthermore, the extent to which results for these
studies represent the nation's housing stock has not been determined. Results are now presented
separately for floors and window sills (with more detailed presentations found in Appendix H).
6.3.1 Post-Intervention Floor Dust-Lead Loadings
Summaries of post-intervention floor (wipe) dust-lead loadings are presented in Table
6-11 according to housing group within each study. According to Table 6-11, all but two of the
19 study groups reported geometric mean or median floor dust-lead loadings at or below 41
ug/ft2 from 6 months to 6 years post-intervention. The other two study groups were from the
Baltimore Experimental Paint Abatement Study, where pre-intervention geometric mean dust-
lead loadings were much greater (556 ng/ft2 and 1261 ng/ft2) than any other study group (at most
58.6 ug/ft2). Eleven study groups reported geometric mean or median floor dust-lead loadings at
or below 21 ug/ft2 at follow-up periods ranging from 12 months to 2 years. Of these 11 groups,
four of the HUD Grantees study groups reported median floor dust-lead loadings at or below 10
ug/ft2 at 12 months post-intervention. Median pre-intervention floor dust-lead loadings in these
four groups ranged from 9 to 26 ug/ft2.
In the HUD Grantees evaluation, seven of the eight largest grantees have median floor
dust-lead loadings at or below 21 ug/ft2 at 12 months post-intervention, compared to a median of
14 ug/ft2 across all grantees. Although pre-intervention floor dust-lead loadings were lower in
the HUD Grantees evaluation compared to other studies, these preliminary results suggest that
floor dust-lead loadings can be maintained at levels below 40 ug/ft2 for at least 12 months post-
intervention.
Results from the Denver CAP study, the Baltimore Follow-up Paint Abatement study, the
Baltimore R&M study, the Boston Interim Dust Intervention study, and the Jersey City CLEAR
study suggest that geometric mean floor dust-lead loadings of below 40 ug/ft2 can be observed
even beyond 12 months post-intervention and up to six years post-intervention, under the same
conditions experienced by the housing units in these studies.
6.3.2 Post-Intervention Window Sill Dust-Lead Loadings
Summaries of post-intervention window sill wipe dust-lead loadings are presented in
Table 6-12 according to housing group. Post-intervention geometric means or medians range
from 24 jig/ft2 to 958 fig/ft2, which are considerably higher than the summaries for floors.
Eleven study groups had geometric mean or median post-intervention window sill dust-lead
loadings below 100 ug/ft2,6 groups were at or below 51 ug/ft2, and 3 groups were at or below 41
ug/ft2.
All but one of the HUD Grantees study groups (the Milwaukee grantee) had median
window sill dust-lead loadings below 100 ^g/ft2 at 12 months post-intervention. As the
intervention strategy for homes in the HUD Grantees evaluation frequently included partial or
272
-------
Table 6-11. Summaries of Pre- and Post-Intervention Floor Wipe Dust-Lead Loadings for
Housing Groups Within Seven Studies
Study
Baltimore
Experimental Paint
Abatement
Studies2
Baltimore Follow-
up Paint
Abatement Study2
Baltimore R&M
Study3
Boston Interim
Dust Intervention
Study2
HUD Grantees4
Denver CAP Study8
Jersey City CLEAR
Study
Study
Group
Study 1
Study 2
1 2-Month Follow-up
19-Month Follow-up
Previously-Abated Units
Units Slated for R&M
Intervention
Automatic Intervention
Randomized Intervention
All Grantees
Baltimore
Boston
Massachusetts
Milwaukee
Minnesota
Rhode Island
Vermont
Wisconsin
Abated Units
Intervention Group
Pro-Intervention Floor
Dust-Lead Loadings1
(pg/ft2)
1261
556
NA
NA
45.6
58,6
33.2
37.3
19
41
24 .
24
14
18
26
28
9
NA
22
Post-Intervention
Floor Dust-Lead Loadings1
Time Following
Intervention
(Months)
6-9
1.5 -3.5 Years
10-14
14-24
4 - 6 Years
24
6
6
12
12
12
12
12
12
12
12
12
2 Years
12
i Summary Value
(pg/ft*)
99
69
20
36
33.0
35.0
23.9
31.4
14
41
18
9
10
18
6
21
5
21.0
15
1 Values are geometric means except for the HUD Grantees studies, where values are medians. "NA" indicates not
available.
* Results are adjusted to reflect total dust-lead loadings by exponentiating the "bioavailable" dust-lead loadings as reported in
the study to the 1.1416 power.
3 Results for the Baltimore R&M Study are converted from BRM dust-lead loadings to wipe-equivalent loadings.
* Data collected through September, 1997
5 Results for the Denver CAP study are converted from CAP cyclone dust-lead loadings to wipe-equivalent loadings.
273
-------
Table 6-12. Summaries of Pre- and Post-Intervention Window Sill Wipe Dust-Lead
Loadings for Housing Groups Within Seven Studies
Study
Baltimore
Experimental Paint
Abatement
Studies2
Baltimore Follow-
up Paint
Abatement Study2
Baltimore R&M
Study3
Boston Interim
Dust Intervention
Study2
HUD Grantees4
Denver CAP
Study5
Jersey City CLEAR
Study
Study
Group
Study 1
Study 2
12-Month Follow-up
19-Month Follow-up
Previously-Abated Units
Units Slated for R&M
Intervention
Automatic Intervention
Randomized Intervention
All Grantees
Baltimore
Boston
Massachusetts
Milwaukee
Minnesota
Rhode Island
Vermont
Wisconsin
Abated Units
Intervention Group
Pro-Intervention SHI
Dust-Lead Loadings1
fe/g/ft2)
15215
2784
NA
NA
163.5
778.4
787
205
258
1191
174
328
264
266
314
147
150
NA
75
Post-Intervention
Sill Dust-Lead Loadings1
Time Following
Intervention
6-9
1.5 -3.5 Years
10-14
14-24
4 - 6 Years
24
6
6
12
12
12
12
12
12
12
12
12
2 Years
12
Summary Value
(TO/*2)
958
199
41
147
97.6
204.9
210
110
90
68
49
50
217
77
85
40
51
66.4
24
1 Values are geometric means except for the HUD Grantees studies, where values are medians. "NA' indicates not
available.
2 Results are adjusted to reflect total dust-lead loadings by exponentiating the "bioavailable' dust-lead loadings as reported in
the study to the 1.1416 power.
3 Results for the Baltimore R&M Study are converted from BRM dust-tead loadings to wipe-equivalent loadings.
* Data collected through September, 1997
* Results for the Denver CAP study are converted from CAP cyclone dust-lead loadings to wipe-equivalent loadings.
274
-------
complete window replacement, these results may not be representative of the outcomes of
interventions prompted by the §403 rule.
Geometric mean window sill dust-lead loadings were below 100 ^fi2 for up to two years
post-intervention in the Baltimore Follow-up Paint Abatement study, Denver CAP study, and
Jersey City CLEAR study. However, in the Baltimore R&M study, Baltimore Experimental
Paint Abatement studies, and Boston Interim Dust Intervention study, geometric mean dust-lead
loadings remain above 100 ug/ft2 over time. In addition, the 19-month follow-up study group
within the Baltimore Follow-up Paint Abatement study and study group #2 of the Baltimore
Experimental Paint Abatement studies suggest that geometric mean dust-lead loadings can dip
, below 100 Mg/ft2 immediately after intervention, but then exceed this level after one year or so.
6.4 SENSITIVITY AND UNCERTAINTY ANALYSES FOR RISK
MANAGEMENT ANALYSES
The following subsections present the results of additional sensitivity and uncertainty
analyses performed to gauge the level of uncertainty in the post-§403 risk estimates (and the
associated decline from baseline estimates) associated with methodological assumptions. These
results should be considered with those presented in the sensitivity and uncertainty analyses in
Section 6.4 of the §403 risk analysis report to characterize overall uncertainty associated with the
methods and assumptions taken in the risk management.
6.4.1 Considering How Baseline Environmental-Lead Levels May
Have Changed Since the HUD National Survey
Section 5.1.4 of this report addressed the sensitivity of the pre-§403 model-based blood-
lead distribution and the resulting health effects and blood-lead concentration endpoint estimates
under the IEUBK and empirical models under different assumptions on how the national
distribution of baseline environmental-lead levels as estimated using HUD National Survey data
may have changed since the time of the survey (1989-1990). The same five sets of adjustments
(i.e., percentage changes) made to the average baseline dust-lead loadings, dust-lead
concentrations, and soil-lead concentrations for each housing unit in the HUD National Survey
were considered in this sensitivity analysis to observe the impact on post-§403 risk estimates
under the following set of example options for standards:
• Average floor dust-lead loading =100 ug/ft2
• Average window sill dust-lead loading = 500 ug/ft2
• Average soil-lead concentration = 2,000 \igfg
• Amount of deteriorated lead-based paint requiring paint maintenance = 5 ft2
• Amount of deteriorated lead-based paint requiring paint abatement - 20 ft2
This set of options was the primary set considered in the sensitivity analyses within Section 6.4
of the §403 risk analysis report.
275
-------
Table 6-13 presents the post-§403 estimates for the health effect and blood-lead
concentration endpoints under both the IEUBK and empirical models, for each of the five sets of
adjustments to the post-§403 environmental-lead levels hi housing units within the HUD
National Survey and under the above assumption on example standards. Also included in this
table are the percentage of homes exceeding the various example standards, which will be lower
than in the §403 risk analysis when declines in the appropriate environmental-lead levels are
considered and higher when increases are considered. The table also lists the baseline risk
estimates for comparison purposes.
Effect on risk analysis: Under the five sets of assumptions involving lower assumed
baseline environmental-lead levels, the percentage of houses that exceed at least one of the
example standards declined by at most about three percentage points (from 21.8% to 18.7%;
Table 6-13), or about three million homes. The assumption that baseline environmental-lead
levels are 25% higher than assumed in the §403 risk analysis results in an increase in the
percentage of homes exceeding at least one standard from 21.8% to 24.1%, an increase of about
2.3 million homes (Table 6-13).
As would be expected, Table 6-13 shows that all assumptions on baseline environmental-
lead levels result in post-§403 estimates of the predicted health effect and blood-lead
concentration endpoints that are lower than baseline (the last column of the table). However, as
the assumed baseline environmental-lead levels become lower in magnitude, the predicted post-
1403 risks actually increase, converging to the baseline estimates. For example, as seen in Table
6-3, baseline lead levels that are 20% below what was assumed in the §403 risk analysis resulted
in an estimated percentage of children with blood-lead concentrations at or above 10 ug/dL of
4.85%, compared to the §403 risk analysis estimate of 4.70%. When baseline lead levels are
50% below the §403 risk analysis estimates, the estimate of this percentage increases to 5.10%.
Such a finding appears counter-intuitive when first reviewing the table. However, the alternative
assumptions being considered hi this sensitivity analysis are to baseline (i.e., pre-§403)
environmental-lead levels. As assumptions on these baseline levels move lower, fewer homes
are triggered by the §403 standards, and the post-§403 distribution of environmental-lead levels
becomes less removed from the baseline distribution. As a result, post-§403 estimates of
predicted health effects and blood-lead concentration are not as different from pre-§403
estimates. In contrast, as assumed baseline environmental-lead levels increase, more homes are
triggered by the §403 standards and, therefore, have their environmental-lead levels drop as a
result of interventions, and lower post-§403 risk estimates relative to baseline are observed.
As seen in Table 6-13, the effect that different assumptions on baseline environmental-
lead levels have on the risk estimates is considerably greater under the IEUBK model than the
empirical model. The percentage of children with blood-lead concentrations at or above 20
ug/dL more than triples under the IEUBK model approach when 50% declines hi both dust-lead
and soil-lead levels were assumed (from 0.054% to 0.166%), compared to a 16% increase under
the empirical model (from 0.406% to 0.469%). Smaller percentage differences are observed for
the other endpoints for both models.
276
-------
Table 6-13. Sensitivity Analysis on How Changes in Household Average Baseline Dust-
Lead Loadings/Concentrations and Soil-Lead Concentration Impact Post-
8403 Estimates of Health Effect and Blood-Lead Concentration Endpoints for
Children Aged 1-2 Years Under a Specified Set of Example Standards1
Assumed Percentage Change In Average Dust-Lead Loadings and Concentrations
(Both Floor and Window Sill) and in Yard-wide Average Soil-Lead Concentration
Dust:
•--:,. . Soil:
.LNo
change
l*to
change
20% ;
decrease
20% ,
decrease.
50%
decrease
', w^> ,''
decrease'
50%
decrease
No.
. change
No
change
. 50%
decrease
Percentage of Homes Exceeding Example Standards/Triggers
Floor Dust
Window Sill Dust
Soil
Interior Paint Maintenance
Exterior Paint
Maintenance
Interior Paint Abatement
Exterior Paint Abatement
Any Standard/Trigger
4.04
12.5
2.49
2.92
3.49
2.43
5.77
21.8
2.34
10.8
1.52
2.92
3.49
2.43
5.77
20.6
0.694
9.10
0.746
2.92
3.49
2.43
5.77
18.7
0.694
9.10
2.49
2.92
3.49
2.43
5.77
18.9
4.04
12.5
0.746
2.92
3.49
2.43
5.77
21.6
25%
increase
. 25%
increase
Baseline
Estimate
{from
Table 5-1
of tiie
§403 risk
analysis
report)
5.68
14.3
3.27
2.92
3.49
2.43
5.77
24.1
Predicted Hearth Effect And Blood-Lead Concentration Endpoints (Based on Empirical Model)
PbB *20 (%)
PbBi10(%)
IQ < 70 (%)
IQ decrement 2 1 (%)
IQ decrement *2 (%)
IQ decrement 23 (%)
Avg. IQ decrement
0.406
4.70
0.110
36.3
9.30
2.93
1.00
0.429
4.85
0.111
36.7
9.53
3.04
1.01
0.469
5.10
0.112
37.3
9.90
3.21
1.03
0.445
4.95
0.111
36.9
9.69
3.11
1.02
0.427
4.84
0.111
36.7
9.51
3.03
1.01
0.378
4.52
0.110
35.9
9.02
2.80
0.995
0.588 •
5.75
0.115
38.5
10.8
3.70
1.06
Predicted Health Effect And Blood-Lead Concentration Endpoints (Based on IEUBK Model)
PbB 220 (%)
PbB2lO(%)
IQ < 70 <%)
IQ decrement 2! (%)
IQ decrement ±2 (%)
IQ decrement a3 (%)
Avg. IQ decrement
0.0539
1.66
0.0984
28.3
4.31
0.858
0.848
0.117
2.48
0.102
31.0
5.77
1.37
0.894
0.166
2.98
0.104
32.7
6.65
1.71
0.924
0.121
2.55
0.102
31.6
5.94
1.42
0.904
0.0681
1.86
0.0992
28.8
4.67
0.983
0.857
0.0542
1.64
0.0982
27.7
4.22
0.847
0.839
0.588
5.75
0.115
38.5
10.8
3.70
1.06
1 Example dust and soil standards were set at: 100 //g/ft2 for floor dust-lead loading, 500 //g/ft2 for window sill
dust-lead loading, and 2,000 jt/g/g for soil-lead concentration. Paint maintenance is performed if more than 5
ft2, but less than 20 ft2 of deteriorated lead-based paint exists. Paint abatement is performed if more than 20
ft2 of deteriorated lead-based paint exists.
277
-------
6.4.2 Impact on the Estimated Incidence of IQ Point Decrement
Assuming Certain Thresholds on the IQ/Blood-Lead Relationship
The sensitivity of baseline and pre-§403 model-based estimates of IQ decrements greater
than 1,2, or 3, and of the average and standard deviation of the distribution of IQ point
decrements was addressed in Section 5.1.5 of this report for various assumptions of a non-zero
threshold of blood-lead concentration on the IQ/blood-lead relationship. The following
thresholds were considered: 1,2, 3,5, 8 and 10 ug/dL. In this section, post-§403 estimates of
these health effect endpoints are estimated (under the same set of options presented in Section
6.4.1, using both the DEUBK and empirical models) under these same alternative blood-lead
concentration thresholds. These estimates are presented in Table 6-14.
Effect on risk analysis: As was also seen in Table 5-7 of this report, Table 6-14 shows
that the post-§403 risk estimates decrease as the assumed blood-lead concentration threshold
increases (i.e., smaller percentages of children experience IQ score decrements under larger
threshold assumptions). The IEUBK model is more sensitive than the empirical model to the
threshold level. For example, the probability of a child experiencing an IQ decrement of at least
1 point decreases by 63% under the IEUBK model (from 28.3% to 10.4%) when the threshold
increases from 0 to 2 ug/dL, compared to only a 52% decrease under the empirical model (from
36.3% to 17.6%). As the assumed threshold increases, the likelihood of experiencing an IQ
decrement of at least 1 point as a result of lead exposure decreases to very low values under both
models, and the average IQ score decrement in the population declines to small fractions of
points.
6.4.3 Considering Alternative Assumptions on Post-Intervention
Dust-Lead Loadings
In the risk management portion (Chapter 6) of the §403 risk analysis report, it was
necessary to make assumptions on predicted post-intervention lead levels when characterizing
the blood-lead concentration and health effect endpoints in a post-§403 environment. These
assumptions were documented in Table 6-2 of the §403 risk analysis report. Among these
assumptions were that dust cleaning activities impacted interior dust-lead loadings in the
following way:
• Post-intervention household average floor (wipe) dust-lead loadings equaled the
minimum of 40 ug/tfand the pre-intervention value.
• Post-intervention household average window sill (wipe) dust-lead loadings
equaled the minimum of 100 ug/ft* and the pre-intervention value.
A dust cleaning was assumed to be included among the interventions performed when either the
floor-dust, window sill-dust, soil, or interior paint abatement standards were exceeded within a
home. These two assumptions on post-intervention dust-lead loadings were made within the
§403 risk analysis based on data reported in EPA's Comprehensive Abatement Performance
278
-------
Table 6-14. Sensitivity Analysis on the Assumed Blood-Lead Concentration Threshold on
IQ Decrement and Its Impact on the Post-§403 Estimates of IQ Decrement
Endpoints for Children Aged 1-2 Years, Under a Specified Set of Example
Standards1
Assumed
Threshold
fc/g/dLl
% of Children Aged 1-2 Years with a Specified IQ
Decrement' Due to Lead Exposure2
IQ Decrement as-,'
T . ,;':
IQ Decrement 2
;]:,:•• 2 '..
IQ Decrement 2
3 ' '
Average IQ
Decrement
{# points)3
Standard
Deviation of IQ
n»».^. ......
-------
study and in the Baltimore Experimental Paint Abatement study (see Section 6.1.2 of the §403
risk analysis report and Section H2.0 of Appendix H of this report).
Tables 6-11 and 6-12 within Section 6.3 of this report presented additional information
on household average (wipe) dust-lead loading at pre- and post-intervention for floors and
window sills, respectively, from several recent lead intervention studies. This information, some
of which was received after the §403 risk analysis report was completed, suggests that it may be
common in some instances to observe household average post-intervention dust-lead loadings
below the assumptions made above, even from 12 months to six years post-intervention. These
findings prompted a sensitivity analysis to investigate how setting assumptions on post-
intervention household average dust-lead loadings to below the 40 ug/ft2 and 100 jug/ft2
specifications would impact the outcome of the risk management analyses.
In this sensitivity analysis, two alternative assumptions on household average post-
intervention floor dust-lead loadings were made: 10 ug/ft2 and 25 fig/ft2. As the geometric mean
(12-month) post-intervention floor dust-lead loading in the HUD Grantees evaluation was 14
Hg/ft2 (Table 6-8) and was even lower for certain grantees, an alternative of 10 ug/ft2 was
selected. The alternative of 25 ug/ft2 for floors was selected as it fell halfway between the
assumptions of 10 and 40 ug/ft2 and was within the range of expected variability in the
summaries for several of the studies in Section 6.3.1.
Similarly, two alternative assumptions on household average post-intervention window
sill dust-lead loadings were made: 50 ug/ft2 and 75 ng/ft2. Evidence from Table 6-12 indicates
that average window sill dust-lead loadings following intervention could approach 50 ug/ft2 in
some instances, especially when floor dust-lead loadings are low. The alternative of 75 ug/ft2
was selected as it fell halfway between the assumptions of 50 and 100 ug/ft2, and it was similar
to the average levels observed by grantees within the HUD Grantees evaluation (although the
HUD Grantees evaluation included window replacement, which was not among the assumed
interventions in the §403 risk analysis).
In the sensitivity analysis, if a given household's pre-intervention average floor dust-lead
loading fell below the given post-intervention assumption, its post-intervention household
average floor dust-lead loading was assumed to be equal to its pre-intervention average (as was
done in Chapter 6 of the §403 risk analysis report). Second, this sensitivity analysis considers
predictions made only by the empirical model, as the IEUBK model does not accept dust-lead
loading as input. Finally, the assumptions made in determining post-intervention soil-lead
concentrations (150 ug/g following soil removal) and amount of deteriorated lead-based paint
(none is present following paint intervention) remained the same as specified in Table 6-2 of the
§403 risk analysis report.
Table 6-15 presents the estimated post-§403 health effect and blood-lead concentration
endpoints associated with the set of example options for standards specified in Section 6.4.1
above, for the alternative assumptions on post-intervention floor and window sill dust-lead
loadings specified above. Note that each alternative assumption is evaluated on its own (i.e., it is
280
-------
•S **
IB*
»
IS-
n*
is
II
CO 10
". •' I***"
CO ID
sg
«•*
is
250
id
V*
CD
.O
CO
CO
CO
in
10
IX
ui
IO
IO
CD
CO
CO
6
CO
o
CO
cn
CO
(O
CM
CO
CO
CM
en
0)
00
CM
o
o
CO
CO
10
0)
id
CO
CO
q
oi
CO
CM
10
cn
cn
CM
00
i3
a
TJ
a
?
281
-S? t
SI
« TJ
« T> d>
C *• -C
t o •*
O 0)
I CL 5
^«
o «?.E 2
-------
the only change from the §403 risk analysis assumptions). In addition, considering the high
correlation in dust-lead loadings between floors and window sills, the two lower alternatives (10
ug/ft2 for floors and 50 ug/ft2 for window sills) and the two higher alternatives (25 fig/ft2 for
floors and 75 fig/ft2 for window sills) are evaluated together. For comparison purposes, post-
intervention estimates under the §403 risk analysis (i.e., assuming 40 fig/ft2 for floors and 100
ug/ft2 for window sills) and the estimates generated under baseline (pre-§403) conditions (both
presented in Table 6-7 of the §403 risk analysis report) are also included in Table 6-15.
Effect on risk analysis. Relative to the results reported hi the §403 risk analysis report
(column 2 of Table 6-15), the greatest deviation occurs with the most substantial change in the
assumptions, i.e., the assumptions of 10 ug/ft2for floors and 50 ug/ft2 for window sills (column 7
of Table 6-15). Under this particular set of alternative assumptions, the percentage of the
nation's children aged 1-2 years that are anticipated to have blood-lead concentration at or above
10 ug/dL following interventions conducted in response to the §403 rule (given the example
standards specified in the footnote to this table) is reduced from 4.70% to 4.53% (a 3.7% decline,
equivalent to approximately 13,700 children12). The corresponding reduction in the percentage
of children with blood-lead concentration at or above 20 ug/dL is from 0.406% to 0.380% (a
6.3% decline, equivalent to approximately 2,000 children).
Under the assumptions of 25 jig/ft2 for floors and 75 ug/ft2 for window sills (column 8 of
Table 6-15), the percentage of the nation's children aged 1-2 years that are anticipated to have
blood-lead concentration at or above 10 ug/dL is reduced from 4.70% to 4.64% (a 1.2% decline,
equivalent to approximately 4,800 children). The corresponding reduction in the percentage of
children with blood-lead concentration at or above 20 ug/dL is from 0.406% to 0.397% (a 2.3%
decline, equivalent to approximately 750 children).
Generally, even lower percentage declines occur for the IQ endpoints compared to the
blood-lead concentration endpoints. The exception occurs with the percentage of children with
IQ decline of at least 3 points, where a 4.2% decline from the §403 risk analysis assumptions was
observed under assumptions of 10 ug/fi2 for floors and 50 Mg/ft2 for window sills.
This sensitivity analysis indicates that while more housing units may achieve reductions
in average dust-lead levels on floors and window sills following a dust cleaning if the assumed
post-intervention floor dust-lead loadings are lowered from those made hi the §403 risk analysis,
the corresponding reduction in the estimated blood-lead concentration and health effect endpoints
appears to be modest, especially when compared to the reduction observed from pre- to post-
§403 conditions.
12 Assuming that 7.96 million children aged 1-2 years reside in the U.S. housing stock (Table 3-3S of the
§403 risk analysis report).
282
-------
6.4.4 Characterizing the Post-Intervention Blood-Lead Distribution
Based on Relative Change from Baseline in the Geometric
Mean and the Probability of a Child's Blood-Lead
Concentration Exceeding 10/ig/dL
As discussed in Section 4.3.1 above and in Appendix Fl of the §403 risk analysis report,
a "scaling algorithm" was used in the §403 risk analysis to characterize the distribution of blood-
lead concentration in the nation's children following interventions that would be performed as a
result of implementing the §403 rule (where the algorithm was applied under a specified set of
example options for the standards, using a specified blood-lead prediction model, and under
assumptions made on the changes in environmental-lead levels that result from the
interventions). This distribution is labeled the "post-§403" distribution. This approach
calculated the geometric mean (GM) and geometric standard deviation (GSD) of the post-§403
blood-lead distribution in the following manner:
( 1)
(2)
where the subscripts indicate the blood-lead distribution which either the GM or the GSD
represents. See Section 4.3.1 for additional information on this approach.
One comment received on the §403 risk analysis was that because the blood-lead
concentration endpoints utilized in the risk analysis were exceedance probabilities (i.e., the
likelihood of a child's blood-lead concentration exceeding a specified value), it was more
important to accurately characterize the right tail of the post-§403 distribution compared to the
remainder of the distribution, especially at blood-lead levels beyond 10 ug/dL. Therefore, a
variant of the scaling approach was considered that involved scaling the probability of a child's
blood-lead concentration exceeding 10 ug/dL rather than the GSD. If P10 was used to represent
this probability, then the alternative scaling algorithm would involve scaling the geometric mean
as in (I) above, but replacing (2) above with the following calculation:
P10post-403 ~P 1 "baseline 0" ^model-based post-403 ' ** "model-based pre-403)
(3)
The resulting value is the estimate of the probability of a child's blood-lead concentration
exceeding 10 ug/dL in a post-§403 environment. It is calculated by multiplying the probability
as calculated in the baseline distribution by the relative change in the probability from the pre-
§403 to post-§403 environment as estimated from model-based blood-lead distributions. Then,
in order to calculate the other blood-lead concentration and health effect endpoints, the GSD of
the post-§403 distribution would be calculated by assuming that this distribution is lognormal.
Therefore,
= exp{(log(10) -
(4)
283
-------
where O"1 denotes the inverse of the standard normal distribution function.
Table 6-16 presents the estimated blood-lead concentration and health effect endpoints
that result when applying this alternative scaling algorithm, under both the IEUBK and empirical
models. The example options for standards that are assumed in this analysis are the same as
those considered in Section 6.4.1 above and are specified in a footnote to Table 6-16. For
comparison purposes, this table also contains the estimates under the original version of the
scaling approach that was utilized in the §403 risk analysis.
Table 6-16. Estimated Post-§403 Health and Blood-Lead Concentration Endpoints Under
the Original and Alternative Scaling Algorithms for Characterizing the Post-
§403 Blood-Lead Distribution
Original scaling algorithm: Geometric mean and GSD are scaled.
Alternative scaling algorithm: Geometric mean and the probability of PbB exceeding 10^/g/dL are scaled.
Health Effect and Hood-Lead
: Concentration Endpoints
% of Children with PbB i 20 A/g/dL
% of Children with PbB 2 10//g/dL
%of Children with IQ < 70 due to
lead exposure
% of Children with IQ decrement 2 1
due to lead exposure
% of Children with IQ decrement 2 2
due to lead exposure
% of Children with IQ decrement 2 3
due to lead exposure
Avg. IQ decrement due to lead
exposure
Geometric Mean PbB (GSD)
Post-1403 Estimates Under the
Risk Management Analysis
(Original Scafing Algorithm)
IEUBK Model
0.0539
1.66
0.0984
28.3
4.31
0.858
0.848
2.74(1.84)
Empirical
Model
0.406
4.70
0.110
36.3
9.30
2.93
1.00
3.03 (2.04)
Post-1403 Estimates Under the
Alternative Scaling Algorithm
IEUBK Model;:
0.156
2.72
0.102
30.1
6.05
1.56
0.884
2.74 (1 .96)
- Empirical
Model 1 :
0.249
3.78
0.107
35.5
8.03
2.24
0.977
3.03(1.96)
Note: Example dust and soil standards were set at: 100 /rg/ft9 for floor dust-lead loading, 500 pg/ft* for window sill dust-
lead loading, and 2.000 ;ig/g for soil-lead concentration. Paint maintenance is performed if more than 5 ft*, but less than
20 ft1, of deteriorated lead-based paint exists. Paint abatement is performed if more than 20 ft2 of deteriorated lead-based
paint exists. GSD = geometric standard deviation. PbB = blood-lead concentration
284
-------
Effect on risk analysis. As indicated in Table 6-16, when the probability of exceeding
10 ug/dL is scaled instead of the GSD, the estimated probability is approximately 64% higher
under the IEUBK model (1.66% to 2.72%), but nearly 20% lower under the empirical model
(4.70% to 3.78%). Note that under the alternative approach, estimates based on the IEUBK and
empirical models are more similar to each other than under the original scaling algorithm. In the
alternative approach, the estimated post-§403 GSD is the same under both models: 1.96. Note
that there was no change in the manner in which the geometric mean blood-lead concentrations
were determined, and therefore, no change is noted between the two approaches.
The above results indicate that the alternative scaling approach has a more significant
impact on the IEUBK model-based estimates compared to the empirical model-based estimates.
The impact of the approach on the empirical model-based estimates is a reduction in the risk
estimates due to a 4% reduction in the estimated GSD, while the impact on IEUBK model-based
estimates is an increase in the risk estimates due to a 6.5% increase in the estimated GSD.
However, because the two approaches did not differ hi how the post-§403 geometric mean blood-
lead level was calculated, the empirical model estimates remain higher than the IEUBK model
estimates.
6.5 LEAD EXPOSURE ASSOCIATED WITH CARPETED FLOOR-DUST
While the §403 proposed rule included a proposed lead hazard standard for dust on
uncarpeted floors, EPA determined that sufficient technical data were not available to direct how
the rule should address lead-contaminated dust on carpeted floors. Based upon public comments
on the proposed rule, EPA is revisiting that determination. This section summarizes the key
findings of statistical analyses on dust-lead loading data for carpeted floors. The analysis had the
following three objectives:
1. Assess the need to have dust-lead on carpeted floors addressed by the §403 rule:
a. Characterize the relationship between floor dust-lead levels and blood-lead
concentration in young children and how this relationship differs for
carpeted and uncarpeted floors (with and without adjusting for the effects
of key demographic variables and for lead levels in other media in which
. standards have been proposed in the §403 rule).
b. Determine the added value of including a carpet dust-lead standard given
the proposed §403 standards for soil, window sills and uncarpeted floors,
or expanding the definition of floors in the rule to include carpeted as well
as uncarpeted floors.
2. Identify appropriate candidates for carpeted floor dust-lead standards and, in
particular, whether one candidate standard should correspond to 50 ug/ft2, the
uncarpeted floor dust-lead standard from the §403 proposed rule.
285
-------
3. Determine whether the wipe technique is acceptable for sampling dust from
carpeted floors for evaluating the risk of lead exposure associated with carpet-
dust, or whether alternative vacuum methods are more appropriate.
A more detailed presentation of the statistical analyses that address these three objectives is
found in Appendix I of this report.
The carpet dust-lead measurement data used in this analysis originated from two lead
exposure studies: the Rochester (NY) Lead-in-Dust study, and the pre-intervention, evaluation
phase of the HUD Lead-Based Paint Hazard Control Grant ("HUD Grantees") Program (data
collected through September, 1997). Both studies were introduced in Section 3.3.1 of the §403
risk analysis report; additional details on these studies that are relevant to this analysis is
presented in Section 13.1 of Appendix I. The results of this analysis, along with relevant findings
documented in EPA's recent literature review report on lead exposure associated with carpets,
furniture, and air ducts (USEPA, 1997b), were used to address the above objectives.
The summary of the analysis results now follows. It is formatted according to the above
three objectives. References to statistical significance are made at the 0.05 level. Unless
otherwise indicated, references to dust-lead loadings are assumed to be for samples collected
using wipe techniques. Section numbers within Appendix I are specified in parentheses where
additional information can be found.
Objective #1: Is there a need to have dust-lead on carpeted floors addressed by the §403
rule?
• Using data collected in the 1997 American Housing Survey, EPA estimates that
approximately 54 million housing units built prior to 1978 contain some wall-to-
wall carpeting. Of these units, wall-to-wall carpeting is found in a living room in
approximately 47 million units and in a bedroom in approximately 46 million
units (i.e., rooms in which children reside and play most frequently, and therefore,
would be targeted in a risk assessment).
• While the §403 proposed rule indicates that lead from floor dust is an important
exposure source for children, the proposed floor dust-lead loading standard was
only relevant for uncarpeted floors. In homes with wall-to-wall carpeting, it is
expected that floor-dust samples in certain rooms can come only from carpeted
floors. While no guidance was given in the §403 proposed rule on a standard to
which risk assessors should compare the results of lead analyses for carpet dust
samples, EPA recognizes (and many commenters on the §403 proposed rule have
noted) that some recommendation for a carpet dust-lead loading standard, based
on using wipe collection techniques, is necessary.
286
-------
• Because children come in frequent direct contact with carpeting when it is present
in their homes, any lead that may be present in carpet dust is likely to be
bioavailable to children.
Objective #1a: Is there any association between carpeted floor dust-lead loadings
and blood-lead concentration?
• For both carpeted and uncarpeted floors in the two studies, the correlation
between household average floor (wipe) dust-lead loading and children's blood-
lead concentration was positive and significantly different from zero. (Sections
14.1.1.1 and 15.1.1.1 of Appendix I)
• No evidence was found in these analyses to suggest that wipe dust-lead loadings
from uncarpeted floors are a better predictor of children's blood-lead
concentration than wipe dust-lead loadings from carpeted floors. (Sections
14.1.1.2,14.1.1.4,15.1.1.2 and 15.1.1.4 of Appendix I)
• No significant difference in the statistical relationship between average floor dust-
lead loading and blood-lead concentration was found between homes with floor
dust sampling conducted from mostly carpeted floors and homes with sampling
from mostly uncarpeted floors. (Sections 14.1.1.3 and 15.1.1.3 of Appendix I)
• Mixed results were found when investigating whether the effect of average
carpeted floor dust-lead loading on blood-lead concentration remained significant
after adjusting for the effects of lead levels in soil, window sill dust, and
uncarpeted floor dust (i.e., other environmental media addressed by the proposed
§403 standards). The carpet dust-lead loading effect was no longer statistically
significant after adjusting for these other effects when analyzing data from the
Rochester study, while the effect remained statistically significant when analyzing
data from the HUD Grantees program evaluation. (Sections 14.1.2 and 15.1.2 of
Appendix I)
' • When interpreted as a whole, these findings provide a powerful argument for
expanding the floor dust-lead standard in the §403 rule to include carpeted floors.
Objective #1 b: Is there any added benefit to adding a carpeted floor dust-lead
loading standard to the proposed §403 standards for lead in soil, window sill dust,
and dust from uncarpeted floors, or to expanding the definition of floors in the rule
to include carpeted floors? (Sections 14.1.3 and 15.1.3 of Appendix I)
• The extent of any added benefit is dependent on the value of the carpet dust-lead
loading standard and the particular criteria being considered in evaluating
performance. Adding a new standard to a set of existing standards will not reduce
sensitivity (i.e., the proportion of homes with elevated blood-lead children that are
287
-------
triggered by the set of standards), but it also will not increase specificity (i.e., the
proportion of homes with no elevated blood-lead children that are not triggered
for an intervention by the standards).
* If the uncarpeted floor dust-lead loading standard of 50 us/ft2 that was proposed
in the §403 proposed rule was extended to included carpeted floors as well, the
resulting performance of the§403 proposed standards (based on the outcome of
performance characteristics analysis) changed little, if any. If a carpeted floor
standard of 40 ug/ft2 was added to the §403 proposed standards, slight
improvements in the performance characteristics were noticed. These findings
were observed regardless of whether or not uncarpeted floors were available to
sample (i.e., whether or not the uncarpeted floor standard was considered).
• Analyses of the Rochester study data indicated that adding a carpet dust-lead
loading standard of approximately 17 fig/ft2 to the proposed §403 standards
considerably improved certain performance characteristics, particularly sensitivity,
without a large decrease in specificity.
• Analysis of the HUD Grantees evaluation data indicated that adding a carpet dust-
lead loading standard of approximately 5 ug/ft2 improved sensitivity and negative
predictive value (NPV, equal to the proportion of homes not triggered for
intervention by the standards that do not contain elevated blood-lead
concentration), but was accompanied by a considerable decrease in specificity. If
the proposed carpet dust-lead loading standard was increased to approximately J3
ug/ft2. this loss of specificity relative to the gains in sensitivity and NPV was
reduced.
• In general, these analyses concluded that expanding the proposed §403 floor dust-
lead standard (of 50 ug/ft2) to encompass both carpeted and uncarpeted floors, or
setting this standard slightly lower at 40 ug/ft2, would not lead to a large decrease
in specificity, but it would tend to result in only minor increases in sensitivity
from what was observed when carpeted floor standards were not being
considered.
Objective #2: If a carpeted floor standard is needed, what should it be? Should it be
different from the proposed uncarpeted floor dust-lead loading standard of 50 //g/ft2?
• The findings listed above for Objective #lb suggest that it may provide an
advantage to have a standard for carpeted floors that is lower than the standard for
uncarpeted floors. (Sections 14.1.1.2,14.2.1,15.1.1.2 and 15.2.1 of Appendix I)
• Having a floor dust-lead loading standard of 40 to 50 ug/ft2 that is expanded to
represent carpeted floors as well as uncarpeted floors would be at least as
protective of children (in terms of the predicted blood-lead concentration at which
288
-------
95% of children exposed at the standard level would be expected to fall below)
than if the standard represented only uncarpeted floors. (Sections 14.2.2 and 15.2.2
of Appendix I)
• When the Rochester study data was used in a performance characteristics analysis
that considered only standards for either carpeted or uncarpeted floors (Sections
14.2.3 and 15.2.3 of Appendix I), a carpeted floor dust-lead loading standard in the
range of 15 to 20 ug/ft2 maximized the total of the four performance
characteristics. In contrast, a standard of 50 fig/ft2 resulted in considerably lower
performance when the standard was for carpeted floors versus uncarpeted floors.
The level of sensitivity achieved by an uncarpeted floor dust-lead loading standard
of 50 ug/ft2 was achieved for carpeted floor dust-lead loading standards below
approximately 33 ug/ft2. However, the uncertainty associated with these estimates
may suggest that these lower levels may not actually differ from a practical
standpoint from the uncarpeted floor dust-lead loading standard in the §403 rule.
Objective #3; What dust sampling method should be used on carpeted floors? (Sections
14.3 and 15.3 of Appendix I)
• The HUD Guidelines (USHUD, 1995) support the use of wipe methods to sample
carpet dust. Participants in the §403 Dialogue Group meetings raised concerns
that requiring widespread use of vacuum techniques for collecting dust samples in
typical risk assessments would be impractical. Therefore, it would be preferable
to allow wipe sampling as an option for collecting dust samples from carpets in a
risk assessment unless wipe techniques were totally unacceptable.
• Different types of dust collection methods can collect different amounts of lead
within a dust sample, especially when sampling from carpets where surface dust is
easier to sample than dust that is deep within the carpet fibers. A laboratory study
done in conjunction with the Rochester study (Emond et al., 1997) concluded that
lead recovery from carpet dust was highest with the BRM vacuum (95.2%)
compared to the wipe (24.4%) and the DVM vacuum (31.4%). For this reason,
different dust collection methods for collecting carpet dust would require different
lead standards to which to compare the results.
• When the wipe method is used on carpets, it tends to collect only dust on the
carpet surface that can readily be removed by the method. This surface dust is
also that which is most likely to come into direct contact with children (USEPA,
1997b).
• Blood-lead concentration tends to be more highly associated with dust-lead
loading than with dust-lead concentration in carpets. (Only dust-lead loadings can
be measured under wipe techniques, while loadings or concentrations can be
measured under vacuum methods.) This contributes to the technical justification
289
-------
that a carpet dust-lead standard would be better conveyed as a loading than as a
concentration.
Each of the three dust collection methods considered in the Rochester study
(BRM vacuum, DVM vacuum, wipe) collected carpet dust samples whose dust-
lead loadings were statistically associated with blood-lead concentration, with the
level of association being similar for each method.
On both carpeted and uncarpeted floors, dust-lead loading measurements from
different dust collection methods were significantly positively correlated. This
suggests that using any of the three methods (including wipe) would portray the
extent of a carpet dust-lead hazard in a similar fashion.
As wipe sampling is currently the method of choice for uncarpeted floors and all
three methods have significant correlations with blood-lead concentration for
carpeted and uncarpeted floors, it is reasonable to develop a carpeted floor dust-
lead loading standard for the wipe sampling method. As this standard would not
apply to vacuum sampled dust-lead loadings, measurements for samples collected
using vacuum techniques could not be directly used in risk assessment via the
§403 rale without first being converted to wipe-equivalent loadings using methods
such as those documented in Section 4.3 of the §403 risk analysis report.
290
-------
7.0 REFERENCES
Abadin, H.G., Wheeler, IS., Jones, D.E., De Rosa, C.T., and MSPH (1997) "A Framework to
Guide Public Health Assessment Decisions at Lead Sites." Journal of Clean Technology,
Environmental Toxicology, and Occupational Medicine. 6(3):225-237.
Altmann, L., Weinsberg, F., Sveinsson, K., Lilienthal, Hi, Wiegand, H., and Winneke, G. (1993)
"Impairment of Long-Term Potentiation and Learning Following Chronic Lead Exposure."
Toxicol. Lett. 66:105-112.
Angell, N.F., and Weiss, B. (1982) "Operant Behavior of Rats Exposed to Lead Before or After
Weaning." Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology. 63:62-71.
Angle, CR., Manton, W.I., and Stanek, K.L. (1995) "Stable Isotope Identification of Lead
Sources in Preschool Children: the Omaha Study." Journal of Toxicology - Clinical
Toxicology. 33(6):657-662.
Angle, C. R., and Mclntire, M. S. (1979) "Environmental Lead and Children: The Omaha
Study." Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health. 5:855-870.
Annest, J.L., and Mahaffey, K. (1984) "Blood Lead Levels for Persons Ages 6 Months - 74
Years: United States, 1976-1980." National Center for Health Statistics, Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention. Vital Health Stat Series No. 11 (233). (PHS) 84-1683.
August 1984.
Baghurst, P.A., McMichael, A.J., Wigg, N.R., Vimpani, G., Robertson, E.F., Roberts, R.J., and
Tong, S. (1992) "Environmental Exposure to Lead and Children's Intelligence at the Age of
Seven Years: The Port Pirie Cohort Study." New England Journal of Medicine. 327:1279-
1284.
Banks, E.C., Ferretti, L.E., and Shucard, D.W. (1997) "Effects of Low Level Lead Exposure on
Cognitive Function in Children: a Review of Behavioral, Neuropsychological and
Biological Evidence." Neurotoxicology. 18(l):237-282.
Barltrop, D., and Meek, F. (1979) Effect of particle size on lead absorption from the gut.
Archives of Environmental Health. 34: 280-285.
Barltrop, D., Strehlow, C.D., Thorton, I., and Webb, J.S. (1974) "Significance of High Soil Lead
Concentrations for Childhood Lead Burdens." Environmental Health Perspectives. 7:75-
82.
291
-------
Bates, M., Malcolm, M., Wyatt, R., Galloway, Y., Gairett, N., Speir, T., and Read, D. (1995)
"Lead in Children from Older Housing Areas in the Wellington Region." New Zealand
Medical Journal, 108(1009):400-404.
Battelle (1996a) "Statistical Evaluation of the Relationship Between Blood-Lead and Dust-Lead
Based on Data from the Rochester Lead-in-Dust Study." Final Report prepared by Battelle
for the Office of Prevention, Pesticides, and Toxic Substances, U.S. Environmental
Protection Agency. Contract No. 68-D2-0139. 22 February 1996.
Battelle (1996b) "Statistical Evaluation of the Relationship Between Blood-Lead and Dust-Lead
Based on Pre-Intervention Data from the R&M Study." Final Report prepared by Battelle
for the Office of Prevention, Pesticides, and Toxic Substances, U.S. Environmental
Protection Agency. Contract No. 68-D2-0139.
Bellinger, D., Stiles, K.M., and Needleman, H. (1992) "Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence,
and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study." Pediatrics. 90(6):855-861.
Bellinger, D., Sloman, J., Leviton, A., Rabinowitz, M., Needleman H.L., and Waternaux, C.
(1991) "Low-Level Lead Exposure and Children's Cognitive Function in the Preschool
Years." Pediatrics. 87(2):219-227.
Bellinger, D.C., Leviton, A., Waternaux, C., Needleman, H., and Rabinowitz, M. (1987)
"Longitudinal Analyses of Prenatal and Postnatal Lead Exposure and Early Cognitive
Development." New England Journal of Medicine. 316:1037-1043.
Bellinger, D.C., Leviton, A., Needleman, H.L., Waternaux, C., and Rabinowitz, M. (1986a)
"Low-Level Lead Exposure and Infant Development in the First Year." Neurobehavioral
Toxicology and Teratology. 8:151-161.
Bellinger, D.C., Leviton, A., Rabinowitz, M., Needleman, H., and Waternaux, C. (1986b)
"Correlates of Low-Level Lead Exposure in Urban Children at 2 Years of Age."
Pediatrics. 77:826-833.
Bellinger, D.C., Leviton, A., Waternaux, C., et al. (1985a) "A Longitudinal Study of the
Developmental Toxicity of Low-Level Lead Exposure in the Prenatal and Early Postnatal
Periods." In: Lekkas TD, ed. International Conference on Heavy Metals in the
Environment, Athens, Greece, September, Vol 1. Edinburgh, United Kingdom: CEP
Consultants, Ltd, 32-34.
Bellinger, D.C., Leviton, A., Waternaux, C., and Allred, E. (1985b) "Methodological Issues in
Modeling the Relationship Between Low-Level Lead Exposure and Infant Development:
Examples from the Boston Lead Study." Environmental Research. 38:119-129.
292
-------
Bellinger, D.C. and Needleman, H.L. (1983) "Lead and the Relationship Between Maternal and
Child Intelligence." Journal of Pediatrics. 102:523-527.
Bhatia, MS., Rai, S., Singbhal, P.K., Nigam, V.R., Bohra, N., and Malik, S.C. (1988) "Pica:
Prevalence and Etiology." Indian Pediatr. 25:1165-1170.
Bornschein, R., Clark, S., Pan, W., and Succop, P. (1990) "Midvale Community Lead Study."
University of Cincinnati.
Bornschein, R. L., Succop, P., Krafft, K., Clark, S., Peace, B., and Hammond, P. B. (1986)
"Exterior Surface Dust Lead, Interior House Dust Lead and Childhood Lead Exposure in an
Urban Environment." Conference in Trace Metals in Environmental Health, Columbia,
MO.
Bornschein, R.L., Succop, P., Dietrich, K.N., Clark, C.S., Que Hee, S., and Hammond, P.B.
(1985a) 'The Influence of Social and Environmental Factors on Dust Lead, Hand Lead, and
Blood Lead Levels in Young Children." Environmental Research. 38:108-118.
Bornschein, R. L., Hammond, P. B., Dietrich, K. N., Succop, P., Krafft, K., Clark, S., Berger, O.,
Pearson, D., and Que Hee, S. (1985b) 'The Cincinnati Prospective Study of Low-Level
Lead Exposure and Its Effects on Child Development: Protocol and Status Report."
Environmental Research, 38:4-18.
Brunekreef, B., Veenstra, S.J., Biersteker, K., and Boleij, J.S.M. (1981) 'The Arnhem Lead
Study: Lead Uptake by 1- to 3- Year-Old Children Living in the Vicinity of a Secondary
Lead Smelter in Amhem, Netherlands." Environmental Research. 25:441-448.
Burger, J., Carruth-Hinchey, C, Ondroff, J., and McMahon M. (1998) "Effects of Lead on
Behavior, Growth, and Survival of Hatchling Slider Turtles." Journal of Toxicology and
Environmental Health. 55:495-502.
Burns, J.M., Baghurst, P.A., Sawyer, M.G., et al. (1999) "Lifetime Low-Level Exposure to
Environmental Lead and Children's Emotional and Behavioral Development: The Port
Pine Cohort Study." American Journal of Epidemiology. 149(8):740-749.
Bushnell, P.J. and Bowman, R.E. (1979a) "Persistence of Impaired Reversal Learning in Young
Monkeys Exposed to Low Levels of Dietary Lead." /. Toxicology and Environmental
Health. 5: 1015-1023.
Bushnell, P.J., and Bowman, R.E. (1979b) "Effects of Chronic Lead mgestion on Social
Development in Infant Rhesus Monkeys." Neurobehavioral Toxicology. 1:207-219.
Bushnell, P.J., Bowman, R.E., Allen, J.R., and Marlar, R.J. (1977) "Scotopic Vision Deficits in
Young Monkeys Exposed to Lead." Science 196:333-335.
293
-------
Butte-Silver Bow Department of Health, and Department of Environmental Health, University of
Cincinnati. (1991) "The Butte-Silver Bow Environmental Health Lead Study." Draft Final
Report.
Calabrese, E., Barnes, R., Stanek, EJ. m, Pastides, H., Gilbert, C.E., Veneman, P., Wang, X.,
Lasztity, A., and Kostecki, P.T. (1989) "How Much Soil Do Young Children Ingest: An
Epidemiological Study." Regulatory Toxicology and Pharmacology. 10:123-137.
Calabrese, E.J., and Stanek, EJ. (1993) "Soil Pica: Not a Rare Event." Journal of
Environmental Sci. Health, A28(2):373-384.
Calabrese, E., Stanek, EJ., and Barnes, R. (1997) "Soil Ingestion Rates in Children Identified
by Parental Observation as Likely High Soil Ingesters." Journal of Soil Contamination.
6(3):271-279.
Calabrese, E., Stanek, EJ., and Gilbert, C.E. (1991) "Evidence of Soil Pica Behavior and
Quantification of Soil Ingested." Human and Experimental Toxicology. 10:245-249.
Calabrese, E., Stanek, EJ., and Gilbert, C.E. (1993) "Lead Exposures in a Soil Pica Child."
J. Environ. Sci. Health. A28 (2):353-362.
Carroll, R.J., and Galindo, C.D. (1999) "Measurement Error, Biases, and the Validation of
Complex Models for Blood Lead Levels in Children." Environmental Health Perspectives.
106 (Supp. 6): 1535-1539.
Carroll R.J., Ruppert, D., and Stefanski. L.A. (1995) Measurement Error in Nonlinear Models.
London: Chapman & Hall.
CDC (1997) "Update: Blood-Lead Levels - United States, 1991-1994." Morbidity and Mortality
Weekly Report. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Public Health Service, U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services. 21 February 1997. 46(7): 141-146.
CDC (1991) "Preventing Lead Poisoning in Young Children: A Statement by the Centers for
Disease Control." Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Public Health Service, U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services. October 1991.
Chen, H., Ma, T., Hume, A. S., and Ho, I. K. (1998) "Developmental Lead Exposure Alters the
Distribution of Protein Kinase C Activity in the Rat Hippocampus." Biomedical and
Environmental Sciences. 11:61-69.
Clark, C.S., Bomschein, R.L., Succop, P., Que Hee, S.S., Hammond, P.B., and Peace, B. (1985)
"Condition and Type of Housing as an Indicator of Potential Environmental Lead Exposure
and Pediatric Blood Lead Levels." Environmental Research. 38:46-53.
294
-------
Clark, S., Bornschein, R.L., Succop, P., Roda, S., and Peace, B. (1991) "Urban Lead Exposures
of Children in Cincinnati, Ohio." Chemical Speciation and Bioavailability. 3:163-171.
Cohen, C.J., Bowers, G.N., and Lepow, MX. (1973) "Epidemiology of Lead Poisoning: A
Comparison Between Urban and Rural Children." Journal of the American Medical
Association. 226:1430-1433.
Cory-Slechta, D.A. (1997) "Relationships Between Pb-Induced Changes in Neurotransmitter
System Function and Behavioral Toxicity." Neurotoxicology. 18(3): 673-688.
Cory-Slechta, D.A., Weiss, B., and Cox, C. (1985) "Performance and Exposure Indices of Rate
Exposed to Low Concentrations of Lead." Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology.
78:291-299.
Cory-Slechta, D.A. and Thompson, T. (1979) "Behavioral Toxicity of Chronic Post-weaning
Lead Exposure in the Rat." Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology. 47:151-159.
Cotter-Howells, J. (1994) "Lead Phosphate Formation in Soils." Environmental Geochemistry
and Health. 16(2): 86.
Cutler, M.G. (1977) "Effects of Exposure to Lead on Social Behavior in the Laboratory Mouse."
Psychopharmacology. 52:279-282.
Davies, DJ.A, Thornton, I., Watt, J.M., Culbard, E.B., Harvey, P.G., Delves, H.T., Sherlock,
J.C., Smart, G.A., Thomas, J.F.A., and Quinn, M.J. (1990) "Lead Intake and Blood Lead in
Two-Year-Old U.K. Urban Children." The Science of the Total Environment. 90:13-29.
Davis, A., Ruby, M.V. and Bergstrom, P.D. (1995) "Factors Controlling Lead Bioavailability in
the Butte Mining District, Montana, USA." Environmental Geochemistry and Health.
16(3-4): 147-157.
Davis, J.M., Otto, D.A., Weil, D.E., and Grant, L.D. (1990) "The Comparative Developmental
Neurotoxicity of Lead in Humans and Animals." Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 12:
215-229.
Davis, S., Waller, P., Buschom, R., Ballou, J., and White, P. (1990) "Quantitative Estimates of
Soil Digestion in Normal Children Between the Ages of 2 and 7 Years: Population-Based
Estimate Using Aluminum, Silicon, and Titanium as Soil Tracer Elements." Archives of
Environmental Health. 45:112-122.
Day, J.C. (1993) Population Projections of the United States, by Age, Sex, Race, and Hispanic
Origin: 2993 to 2050. U.S. Bureau of the Census, Current Population Reports P25-1104.
295
-------
Dersimonian, R. and Laird, N. (1986) "Mela-Analysis in Clinical Trials." Controlled Clinical
Trials. 7:177-188.
Diemel, J.A., Bnmekreef, B., Boleij, J.S., Biersteker, K. and Veenstra, SJ. (1981) "The Arnhem
Lead Study H Indoor Pollution, and Indoor/Outdoor Relationships." Environmental
Research. 25:449-456.
Dieter, M.P, Matthews, H.B., Jeffcoat, R.A. and Moseman, R.F. (1993) "Comparison of Lead
Bioavailability in F344 Rats Fed Lead Acetate, Lead Oxide, Lead Sulfide or Lead Ore
Concentrate from Skagway, Alaska." Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health.
39(1): 79-93.
Dietrich, K.N., Berger, O.G., Succop, P.A., Hammond, P.B., and Bornschein, R.L.. (1993) "The
Developmental Consequences of Low To Moderate Prenatal and Postnatal Lead Exposure:
Intellectual Attainment in the Cincinnati Lead Study Cohort Following School Entry."
Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 15:37-44.
Duggan, M.J. and Inskip, M.J. (1985) "Childhood Exposure to Lead in Surface Dust and Soil: A
Community Health Problem." Public Health Reviews. 13: 1-54.
Elhelu, M. A., Caldwell, D., and Hirpassa, W. (1995) "Lead in Inner-City Soil and Its Possible
Contribution to Children's Blood Lead." Archives of Environmental Health. 50(2): 165-
169.
Emond, M.J., Lanphear, B.P., Watts, A., Eberly, S., and Members of the Rochester Lead-in-Dust
Study Group. (1997) "Measurement Error and Its Impact on the Estimated Relationship
Between Dust Lead and Children's Blood Lead." Environmental Research 72:82-92
Emhart, C.B., Morrow-Tlucak, M., Worf, A.W., Super, D., and Drotar, D. (1989) "Low Level
Lead Exposure in the Prenatal and Early Preschool Periods; Intelligence Prior to School
Entry." Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 11:161-70.
Farfel, M.R. and Chisolm, J.J., Jr. (1991) "An Evaluation of Experimental Practices for
Abatement of Residential Lead-Based Paint: Report on a Pilot Project." Environmental
Research. 55:199-212.
Farfel, M.R. and Chisolm, J.J., Jr. (1990) "Health and Environmental Outcomes of Traditional
and Modified Practices for Abatement of Residential Lead-Based Paint." American
Journal of Public Health. 80(10): 1240-1245.
Fleiss, J.L. (1981) Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. Second Edition. New York:
John Wiley & Sons. Section 1.2, pp. 4-9.
296
-------
Fox, D.A., Campbell, M.L., and Blocker, Y.S. (1997) "Functional Alterations and Apoptotic
Cell Death in the Retina Following Developmental or Adult Lead Exposure."
Neurotoxicology 18(3):645-664.
Francek, M.A., Makimaa, B., Pan, V., and Hanko, J.H. (1994) "Small Town Lead Levels: A Case
Study from the Homes of Pre-schoolers in Mt. Pleasant, Michigan." Environmental
Research. 84(2): 159-166.
Freeman, G.B., Johnson, J.D., Killinger, J.M., Liao, S.C., Feder, P.I., Davis, A.O., Ruby, M.V.,
Channey, R.L., Lovre, S.C., and Bergstrom, P.D. (1992) "Relative Bioavailability of Lead
from Mining Waste Soil in Rats." Fund. Appl. ToxicoL 19:399-398.
Fulton, M., Raab, G., Thomson, G., Laxen, D., Hunter, R., and Hepburn, W. (1987) "Influence of
Blood Lead on the Ability and Attainment of Children in Edinburgh." Lancet. 1:1221-
1226.
Galke, W. A., Hammer, D.I., and Lawrence, S. W. (1975) "Environmental Determinants of Lead
Burdens in Children." In: International Conference on Heavy Metals in the Environment-
Symposium Proceedings, T. C. Hutchinson, S. Epstein, A. L Page, J. VanLoon and T.
Davey (eds.), Institute for Environmental Studies, Toronto, ON, Canada, 3:53-74.
Gallacher, J.E., Elwood, P.C., Phillips, K.M., Davies, B.E., and Jones, D.T. (1984) "Relation
Between Pica and Blood Lead hi Areas of Differing Lead Exposure." Archives of Diseases
in Childhood. 59(1): 4(M4.
Geist, C.R., Balko, S.W., Morgan, M.E., and Angiak, R. (1985) "Behavioral Effects Following
Rehabilitation from Postnatal Exposure to Lead Acetate." Percept. Mot. Skills. 60:527-
536.
Gilbert, S.G., and Rice, D.C. (1987) "Low-level Lifetime Lead Exposure Produces Behavioral
Toxicity (Spatial Discrimination Reversal) in Adult Monkeys." Toxicology and Applied
Pharmacology. 91:484-490.
Goldstein, G. (1992) "Developmental Neurobiology of Lead Toxicity." In Human Lead
Exposure (Needleman, H., ed.). 125-135.
Goldstein, G. (1990) "Lead Poisoning and Brain Cell Function." Environ. Health Perspect.
89:91-94.
Greene, T., Ernhart, C.B., and Boyd, T.A. (1992) "Contributions of Risk Factors to Elevated
Blood and Dentine Lead Levels in Preschool Children." The Science of the Total
Environment. 115:239-260.
297
-------
Harrison, G. (1987) "A Survey of the Lead Distribution in the Soil of Corpus Christi, Texas."
Texas Journal of Science. 39(1): 15-22.
Harvey, P.O., Hamlin, M.W., Kumar, R., Morgan, G., Spurgeon, A., and Delves, H.T. (1988)
"Relationship Between Blood Lead, Behaviour, Psychometric and Neuropsychological Test
Performance in Young Children." British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 29:43-52.
Hastings, L., Cooper, G.P., Bomschein, R.L., and Michaelson, LA, (1977) "Behavioral Effects
of Low Lead Neonatal Lead Exposure." Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior. 7: 37-
42.
Hatzakis, A., Kolevi, A., Katsouyanni, K., et al. (1987) "Psychometric Intelligence and
Attentional Performance Deficits in Lead Exposed Children." International Conference on
Heavy Metals in the Environment. Edinburgh, Scotland; CEP Consultants. 16:57-67.
Hatzakis, A., Kokkeni, A., Maranelias, C., Katsouyanni, K., Salaminios, F., Kalandidi, A., et al.
(1989) "Psychometric Intelligence Deficits in Lead-exposed Children." In: Lead Exposure
and Child Development (Smith MA., Grant, LD., Sora Al, eds.). London: Kluwer,
1989:211-23.
Hawk, B.A., Schroeder, S.R., Robinson, G., et al. (1986) "Relation of Lead and Social Factors to
IQ of Low-SES Children: A Partial Replication." American Journal of Mental Deficiency.
91:178-183.
Karam, H.S., and Beck, B.D. (1990) "Current Issues in Determining Acceptable Lead
Concentrations in Soils." Comments on Toxicology. 3(6):509-529.
Kaufman, A.S. (1996) "IQ, Lead Level, and Inferences from Research Studies: Comments
Addressing the Underlying Science Forming the Basis of HUD's Regulatory Impact
Analysis." Submitted to the Environmental Health Committee of the Science Advisory
Board, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, in their technical review of the proposed
TSCA Section 403 regulations (Identification of Dangerous Levels of Lead). August 1996.
Kenny, D.A. (1979) Correlation and Causality. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Chapter 1,
pp. 1-12.
Krueger, J. A. and Duguay, K. M. (1989) "Comparative Analysis of Lead in Maine Urban Soils."
Bulletin of Environmental Contamination and Toxicology. 42:574-581.
Kuhlmann, A.C., McGlothan, J.L., and Guilarte, T.R. (1997) "Developmental Lead Exposure
Causes Spatial Learning Deficits in Adult Rats." Neuroscience Newsletters. 233:101-104.
Lacey, E.P. (1990) "Broadening the Perspective of Pica: Literature Review." Public Health
Reports. 105(1 ):29-35.
298
-------
Lanphear, B.P., Matte, T.D., Rogers, J., Clickner, R.P., Dietz, B., Bornschein, R.L., Succop, P.,
Mahaffey, K.R., Dixon, S., Galke, W., Rabinowitz, M., Farfel, M., Rohde, C, Schwartz, J.,
Ashley, P., and Jacobs, D. (1998) 'The Contribution of Lead-Contaminated House Dust
and Residential Soil to Children's Blood Lead Levels: A Pooled Analysis of 12
Epidemiologic Studies." Environmental Research (Section A). 79:51-68.
Lanphear, B.P., Weitzman, M., and Eberly, S. (1996a) "Racial Differences in Urban Children's
Environmental Exposures to Lead." American Journal of Public Health. 86(10): 1460-1463.
Lanphear, B.P., Weitzman, M., Winter, N.L., Eberly, S., Yakir, B., Tanner, M., Emond, M., and
Matte, T.D. (1996b) "Lead-Contaminated House Dust and Urban Children's Blood Lead
Levels." American Journal of Public Health. 86(10): 1416-1421.
Lansdown, R., Yule, W., Urbanowicz, M.A., et al. (1986) "The Relationship Between Blood
Lead Concentrations, Intelligence, Attainment and Behavior hi a School Population: The
Second London Study." International Archives of Occupational and Environmental
Health. 57:225-235.
Laperche, V., Logan, T.J., Gaddam, P., and Traina, S.J. (1997) "Effect of Apatite Amendments
on Plant Uptake of Lead from Contaminated Soil." Environmental Science and
Technology. 31(10): 2745-2753.
Lasky, R.E., Maier, M.M., Snodgrass, E.B., Hecox, K.E., and Laughlin, N.K. (1995) "The
Effects of Lead on Otoacoustic Emissions and Auditory Evoked Potentials in Monkeys."
Neurotoxicology and Teratology 17(6):633-644.
Lilienthal, H., Kohler, K., Turfeld, M., and Winneke, G. (1994) "Persistent Increases in Scotopic
B-wave Amplitudes After Lead Exposure in Monkeys." Exp. Eye Res. 59:203-209.
Lilienthal, H., Winneke, G., and Ewert, T. (1990) "Effects of Lead on Neurophysiological and
Performance Measures: Animal and Human Data." Environmental Health Perspectives.
89:21-25.
Lyngbye, T., Hansen, O.N., and Grandjean, P. (1990) "Predictors of Tooth Lead Level with
Special Reference to Traffic: A Study of Lead-Exposure in Children." International
Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health. 62(6):417-422.
Marais, M.L., and Wecker, W.E. (1998) "Correcting for Omitted Variables and Measurement
Error Bias in Regression with an Application to the Effect of Lead on IQ." Journal of the
American Statistical Association. 93:494-505.
McKinney, J. (1993) "Metals Bioavailability and Disposition Kinetics Research Needs
Workshop, July 18-19, 1990." Toxicological and Environmental Chemistry. 38: 1-71.
299
-------
Mello, C.F., Kraemer, C.K., Filippin, A., Morsch, V.M., Rodrigues, A.L., Martins, A.F., and
Rubin, M.A. (1998) "Effect of Lead Acetate on Neurobehavioral Development of Rats."
Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research. 31(7): 943-950.
Mielke, H. W. (1995) "Lead in New Orleans Soils: New Images of an Urban Environment."
Environmental Geochemistry and Health. 16(3/4): 123-128.
Mielke, H. W. (1993) "Lead Dust Contaminated U.S.A. Communities: Comparison of Louisiana
and Minnesota." Applied Geochemistry. Supp. Issue No. 2,257-261.
Mielke, H. W., Adams, J. E., Huff, B., Pepersack, J., Reagan, P. L., Stoppel, D., and Mielke, P.
W. (1992) "Dust Control as a Means of Reducing Inner-City Childhood Pb Exposure." In:
Trace Substances in Environmental Health - XXV. Supplement to Volume 14,
Environmental Geochemistry and Health (B. D. Beck, ed.) 121-128.
Mielke, H. W., Adams, J. L., Reagan, P. L., and Mielke, P. W. (1989) "Soil-Dust Lead and
Childhood Lead Exposure as a Function of City Size and Community Traffic Flow: The
Case for Lead Abatement in Minnesota." In: Lead in Soil: Issues and Guidelines,
Supplement to Volume 9 of Environmental Geochemistry and Health (B. E. Davis and B. G.
Wixson, eds.). 253-271.
Mielke, H. W., Anderson, J.C., Berry, K. J., Mielke, P. W., Chaney, R. L., and Leech, M. (1983)
"Lead Concentrations in Inner-City Soils as a Factor in the Child Lead Problem." American
Journal of Public Health. 73(12): 1366-1369.
Nagymajtenyi, L., Desi, L, Schulz, H., and Papp, A. (1998) "Consequences of Lead Exposure of
Rats During Pregnancy, Lactation, and Postweaning. A Combined Behavioural and
Neurotoxicological Study." International Journal of Environmental Health Research.
8(2): 121-135.
Needleman, H.L. (1998) "IQ, Lead Level, and Inferences from Research Studies." Comments to
the Environmental Health Committee of the Science Advisory Board, U.S. Environmental
Protection Agency, in their technical review of the proposed TSC A Section 403 regulations
(Identification of Dangerous Levels of Lead). 2 September 1998.
Needleman, H.L., and Gatsonis, C.A. (1990) "Low-Level Lead Exposure and the IQ of
Children." Journal of the American Medical Association. 263(5):673-678.
Otto, D.A., and Fox, D.A. (1993) "Auditory and Visual Dysfunction Following Lead Exposure."
Neurotoxicology. 14(2-3): 191-208.
Petit, T.L., Alfano, D.P., and LeBoutillier, J.C. (1983) "Early Lead Exposure and the
Hippocampus: a Review and Recent Advances." Neurotoxicology. 4(l):79-94.
300
-------
Pendleton, S. (personal communication to Brad Schultz, U.S. EPA).
Pocock, S.J., Smith, M., and Baghurst, P. (1994) "Environmental Lead and Children's
Intelligence: A Systematic Review of the Epidemiological Evidence." BMJ. 309:1189-
1197.
Que Hee, S., Peace, B., Clark, S., Boyle, J. R., Bomschein, R. L., and Hammond, P. B. (1985)
"Evolution of Efficient Methods to Sample Lead Sources, Such as House Dust and Hand
Dust, in the Homes of Children." Environmental Research. 38:77-95.
Rabinowitz, M.B., Wang, J.D., and Soong, W.T. (1992) "Apparent Threshold of Lead's Effect
on Child Intelligence." Bulletin of Environmental Contamination and Toxicology. 48:688-
695.
Rabinowitz, M., Leviton, A., Needleman, H., Bellinger, D., and Waternaux, C. (1985)
"Environmental Correlates of Infant Blood Lead Levels in Boston." Environmental
Research. 38:96-107.
Rice, D. C. (1996) "Behavioral Effects of Lead: Commonalities Between Experimental and
Epidemiologic Data." Environmental Health Perspectives. 104 (Suppl. 2): 337-351.
Rice, D.C. (1988) "Chronic Low-Lead Exposure Does Not Affect Simple Reaction Time."
Neurotoxicology. 9:105-108.
Rice, D.C. (1985) "Chronic Low-Lead Exposure from Birth Produces Deficits in Discrimination
Reversal in Monkeys." Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology. 77:201-210.
Rice, D.C. and Gilbert, S.G, (1990a) "Sensitive Periods for Lead-Induced Behavioral
Impairment (Nonspatial Discrimination Reversal) in Monkeys." Toxicology and Applied
Pharmacology. 102:101-109.
Rice, D. C. and Gilbert, S.G. (1990b) "Lack of Sensitive Period for Lead-Induced Behavioral
Impairment on a Spatial Delayed Alternation Task in Monkeys." Toxicology and Applied
Pharmacology. 103:364-373.
Rieuwerts, J.S., and Farago, M.E. (1995) "Lead Contamination in Smelting and Mining
Environments and Variations in Chemical Forms and Bioavailability." Chemical
Speciation and Bioavailability. 7(4): 113-123.
Roberts, J.W., Han, W., and Ruby, M.G. (1996) "Project Summary: Evaluation of Dust
Samplers for Bare Floors and Upholstery." National Exposure Laboratory, Office of
Research and Development, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. EPA/600/R-96-001.
January 1996.
301
-------
Roels, H.A., Buchet, J.P., Lauwerys, R.R., Bruaux, P., Claeys-Thoreau, F., LaFontaine, A., and
Verduyn, G. (1980) "Exposure to Lead by the Oral and the Pulmonary Routes of Children
Living in the Vicinity of a Primary Lead Smelter." Environmental Research. 22:81-94.
Romieu, I., Carreon, T., Lopez, L., Palazuelos, E., Rios, C., Manuel, Y., and Hemandez-Avila,
M. (1995) "Environmental Urban Lead Exposure and Blood Lead Levels in Children of
Mexico City." Environmental Health Perspectives. 103(11): 1036-1040.
Schmitt, M.D.C., Trippler, D.J., Wachtler, J.N. and Lund, G.V. (1988) "Soil Lead
Concentrations in Residential Minnesota as Measured by ICP-AES." Water, Air, and Soil
Pollution. 39:157-168.
Schroeder, S.R., Hawk, B., Otto, D., Mushak, P., and Hicks, R.E. (1985) "Separating the Effects
of Lead and Social Factors on IQ." Environmental Research. 91:178-183.
Schwartz, J. (1994) "Low-Level Lead Exposure and Children's IQ: A Meta-Analysis and Search
for a Threshold." Environmental Research. 65:42-55.
Schwartz, J. (1993) "Beyond LOEL's, p Values, and Vote Counting: Methods for Looking at the
Shapes and Strengths of Association." Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 14(2-3):237-246.
Sedman, R.M. (1989) "The Development of Applied Action Levels for Soils Contact: A
Scenario for the Exposure of Humans to Soils in a Residential Setting." Environmental
Health Perspectives. 79:291-313.
Sedman, R.M., and Mahmood, RJ. (1994) "Soil Ingestion by Children and Adults Reconsidered
Using the Results of Recent Tracer Studies." Journal of Air & Waste Management
Association. 44:141-144.
Shellshear, I.D., Jordon, L.D., Hogan, D.J., and Shannon, F.T. (1975) "Environmental Lead
Exposure in Christchurch Children: Soil Lead as a Potential Hazard." New Zealand Med.
J. 81:382-386.
Sheppard, S.C. (1998) "Geophagy: Who Eats Soils and Where Do Possible Contaminants Go?"
Environmental Geology (Berlin). 33(2-3): 109-114.
Silbergeld, E.K. (1991) "Aneurological Perspective on Lead Toxicity." In Human Lead
Exposure (Needleman, H.L. ed.). Boca Raton, Florida: CRC Press, 89-103.
Silva, P.A., Hughes, P., Williams, S., and Faed, J.M. (1988) "Blood Lead, Intelligence, Reading
Attainment, and Behaviour in Eleven Year Old Children in Dunedin, New Zealand."
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 29:43-52.
302
-------
Simon, S.L. (1998) "Soil Ingestion by Humans: A Review of History, Data, and Etiology with
Application to Risk Assessment of Radioactively Contaminated Soils." Health Physics,
74(6):647-672.
Stanek, E.J. m, and Calabrese, EJ. (1995) Daily Estimates of Soil Ingestion in Children.
Environmental Health Perspectives. 103:276-285.
Stanek E.J., Calabrese, E.J., Mundt, K., Pekow, P., and Yeatts, K.B. (1998) "Prevalence of Soil
Mouthing/Ingestion Among Healthy Children Aged 1 to 6." Journal of Soil
Contamination. 7(2):227-242.
Stark, A. D., Quah, R. F., Meigs, J. W., and DeLouise, E. R. (1982) "The Relationship of
Environmental Lead to Blood-Lead Levels in Children." Environmental Research. 27:372-
383.
Steuteville, W.D. (1990) "Interior Remediation of Residences: Lead Abatement (A Case
History of a Superfund Project and Public Involvement)." In: Procedures of the National
Conference of Hazardous Wastes and Hazardous Materials. Silver Springs, MD:
Hazardous Materials Research Control Institute.
Sutton, P. Athanasoulis, M., Ressel P., Guirguis, G., Haan, M., Schlag, R., and Goldman, L.
(1995) "Lead Levels in the Household Environment of Children in Three High-Risk
Communities in California." Environmental Research. 68:45-57.
Teichmean, J., Coltrin, D., Prouty, K., and Bir, W. A. (1993) "A Survey of Lead Contamination
in Soil Along Interstate 880, Alameda County, California." Journal of the American
Industrial Hygiene Association. 54(9): 557-559.
Tong, S. (1998) "Lead Exposure and Cognitive Development: Persistence and a Dynamic
Pattern." J. Pediatr. Child Health. 34:114-118.
Tong, S. T. (1990) "Roadside Dusts and Soils Contamination in Cincinnati, Ohio."
Environmental Management. 14(1): 107-114.
Tong, S., Baghurst, P., McMichael, A., Sawyer, M., and Mudge, J. (1996) "Lifetime Exposure to
Environmental Lead and Children's Intelligence at 11-13 Years: the Port Pirie Cohort
Study." British Medical Journal. 312:1569-1575.
U.S. Census Bureau (1999) Current Housing Reports, Series HI50/97, American Housing
Survey for the United States: 1997 (Table 2-1). U.S. Government Printing Office,
Washington, DC 20222.
303
-------
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1999) Toxicological Profile for Lead. Update,
1999. Atlanta: Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry, Division of
Toxicology/Toxicology Information Branch, U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1992) Impact of Lead-Contaminated Soil on
Public Health. Atlanta: Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry, Division of
Toxicology/Toxicology Information Branch, U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services. May 1,1992.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (1991) "Philadelphia Neighborhood Lead
Study." Final Report. Atlanta: Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry,
Division of Toxicology/Toxicology Information Branch, U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services.
U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development (1998) "Evaluation of the HUD Lead-
Based Paint Hazard Control Grant Program: Fifth Interim Report, Progress as of September
1,1997." Prepared by the National Center for Lead Safe Housing and the University of
Cincinnati Department of Environmental Health for the U.S. Department of Housing and
Urban Development. March 1998.
U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development (1995a) "The Relation of Lead-
Contaminated House Dust and Blood Lead Levels Among Urban Children." Volumes I
and H. Final Report to the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development Grant
from the Departments of Pediatrics, Biostatistics, and Environmental Medicine, The
Rochester School of Medicine, Rochester, New York, and The National Center for Lead-
Safe Housing, Columbia Maryland.
U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development (1995b) "Guidelines for the Evaluation
and Control of Lead-Based Paint Hazards in Housing." Office of Lead-Based Paint
Abatement and Poisoning Prevention.
U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development (1991) "The HUD Lead-Based Paint
Abatement Demonstration (FHA)." Final Report, Office of Policy Development and
Research, U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1998a) "Risk Analysis to Support Standards for Lead in
Paint, Dust, and Soil." Office of Pollution Prevention and Toxics, U.S. Environmental
Protection Agency. EPA 747-R-97-006.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1998b) "An SAB Report: Technical Review of the
Proposed TSCA Section 403 Regulation (Identification of Dangerous Levels of Lead)."
Environmental Health Committee, Science Advisory Board, U.S. Environmental Protection
Agency. EPA-SAB-EHC-99-004, November 1998.
304
-------
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1998c) "Analysis of Pathways of Residential Lead
Exposure in Children." Office of Prevention Pesticides, and Toxic Substances. EPA 747-
R-98-007. 27 April 1998 (peer review version).
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1997a) "Lead Exposure Associated with Renovation
and Remodeling Activities: Environmental Field Sampling Study. Volume I: Technical
Report." Office of Pollution Prevention and Toxics, U.S. Environmental Protection
Agency. EPA 747-R-96-007.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1997b) "Summary and Assessment of Published
Information on Determining Lead Exposures and Mitigating Lead Hazards Associated with
Dust and Soil in Residential Carpets, Furniture, and Forced Air Ducts." Office of
Prevention Pesticides, and Toxic Substances, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. EPA
747-S-97-001.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1997c) "Lead-Based Paint Abatement and Repair and
Maintenance Study in Baltimore: Finding Based on Two Years of Follow-Up." Office of
Prevention Pesticides, and Toxic Substances, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. EPA
747-R-97-005.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1996a) "Urban Soil Lead Abatement Demonstration
Project, Volume I: Integrated Report." Office of Research and Development, U.S.
Environmental Protection Agency. EPA 600-P-93-001aF, April 1996.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1996b) "Comprehensive Abatement Performance
Study. Volume I: Summary Report." Office of Prevention, Pesticides, and Toxic
Substances, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. EPA 230-R-94-013a.
U. S. Environmental Protection Agency (1996c) "Lead-Based Paint Abatement and Repair and
Maintenance Study in Baltimore: Pre-Intervention Findings." EPA 747-R-95-012, August
1996.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1995) "Report on the National Survey of Lead-Based
Paint in Housing. Base Report." Office of Pollution Prevention and Toxics, U.S.
Environmental Protection Agency. EPA747-R95-003.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1994) "Guidance Manual for the Integrated Exposure
Uptake Biokinetic Model for Lead in Children." Office of Emergency and Remedial
Response, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. EPA/540/R-93/081, PB93-963510.
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (1986) "Air Quality Criteria for Lead, Volume IV of IV."
Research Triangle Park NC: Environmental Criteria and Assessment Office, U.S.
Environmental Protection Agency. EPA-600/8-83/028dF. June 1986.
305
-------
Viverette, L., Mielke, H.W., Brisco, M, Dixon, A., Schaefer, J., and Pierre, K. (1996)
"Environmental Health in Minority and Other Underserved Populations: Benign Methods
for Identifying Lead Hazards at Day Care Centres of New Orleans." Environmental
Geochemistry and Health. 18(1): 41-45.
Walsh, T.J., and Tilson, H.A. (1984) "Neurobehavioral Toxicology of the Organoleads."
Neurotoxicology. 5:67-86.
Wang, L., Xu S.E., Zhang, G.D, and Wang, W.Y. (1989) "Study of Lead Absorption and its
Effect on Children's Development." Biomedical and Environmental Sciences. 2:325-330.
Weitzman, M., Aschengrau, A., Bellinger, D., Jones, R., Hamlin, J.S., and Beiser, A. (1993)
"Lead-Contaminated Soil Abatement and Urban Children's Blood Lead Levels." Journal of
the American Medical Association. 269(13): 1647-1654.
Westat (1998) "A Report on the Analysis of the Blood Lead-Environmental Lead Relationship."
Report for the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development. 7 August 1998.
Winneke, G., Lilienthal, H., and Kramer, U. (1996) "The Neurobehavioural Toxicology and
Teratology of Lead." Arch Toxicol Suppl. 18:57-70.
Winneke, G., Brockhaus, A., Ewers, V., Kramer, V., and Neuf, M. (1990) "Results from the
European Multicenter Study on Lead Neurotoxicity in Gulden: Implications for Risk
Assessment." Neurotoxicol Teratol 12:553-559.
Winneke, G., Beginn, U., Ewert, T., et al. (1985) "Comparing the Effects of Perinatal and Later
Childhood Lead Exposure on Neurophysiological Outcome." Environmental Research.
38:155-167.
Yule, W., Lansdown, R., Millar, L, and Urbanowiez, M. (1981) "The Relationship Between
Blood Lead Concentration, Intelligence, and Attainment in a School Population: a Pilot
Study." DevMed Child Neuro. 23:567-576.
306
-------
50272-101
REPORT DOCUMENTATION
PAGE
1. REPORT NO.
EPA 747-R-00-004
3. Recipient's Accession No.
4. Title and Subtitle
Risk Analysis to Support Standards for Lead in Paint, Dust, and Soil: Supplemental Report
5. Report Date
December 2000
6.
7. Author(s) Lordo, RA, Burgoon, DA, Ma, ZJ, Matthews, MC, Menkedick, JG, Naber, S, Nahhas,
R, Neighbor, M, Niemuth, NA, Wood, BJ, Wooton, M
8. Performing Organization
Rept. No.
9. Performing Organization Name and Address
Battelle Memorial Institute
505 King Avenue
Columbus, Ohio 43201-2693
10. ProjecVTask/Work Unit No.
0476101-20
11. Contract(C) or Grant(G) No,
(C| 68-W-99-033
12. Sponsoring Organization Name and Address
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency
Office of Pollution Prevention and Toxics
401 M Street, S.W.
Washington, D.C. 20460
13. Type of Report & Period
Covered
Technical Report
14.
15. Supplementary Notes
Support staff involved in the risk analysis and the production of this report included Haisey Boyd, Ying-Liang Chou. and Kensuke
Shirakawa.
16. Abstract (Limit 200 words)
Title X of the Housing and Community Development Act, known as the Residential Lead-Based Paint Hazard Reduction Act of 1992,
contains legislation designed to evaluate and reduce exposures to lead in paint, dust, and soil in the nation's housing. As part of Title X,
the Toxic Substances Control Act (TSCA) was amended to include Title IV, 'Lead Exposure Reduction'. Section 403 of TSCA requires
EPA to define standards for lead in paint, dust, and soil. Federal, state, and local public health agencies, as well as private property
owners and other private sector interests, will use these standards to determine in which homes actions should be taken to reduce or
prevent the threat of childhood lead poisoning.
This report is a supplement to EPA 747-R-97-006 (June 1998), which documented the outcome of a risk analysis that supported EPA's
efforts to propose standards in response to Section 403. The additional literature reviews, data summaries, and data analyses presented
in this report focus on selected topics raised in comments on the risk analysis that were made by EPA's Science Advisory Board and the
general public. EPA has cited the findings of this report when preparing responses to these comments.
17. Document Analysis
a. Descriptors
Blood-lead concentration, childhood health risks, adverse health effects, dust-lead loading, soil-lead concentration, lead hazards,
lead-based paint, risk assessment, risk characterization, risk management
b. Identifiers/Open-Ended Terms
abatement, intervention, hazard identification, exposure assessment, dose-response assessment, HUD National Survey,
Rochester Lead-in-Dust Study, Evaluation of the HUD Lead-Based Paint Hazard Control Grant Program, NHANES III,
bioavailability, modeling, performance characteristics analysis, sensitivity and uncertainty analysis. Section 403, Title X
C. COSATl Field/Group
18. Availability Statement
Release Unlimited
19. Security Class (This Report)
Unclassified
2O. Security Cfass (This Page)
Unclassified
21 . No. of Pa
340 (Vol.
ges
I); 232 (Vol. II)
22. Price
(See ANSI-239.18)
OPTIONAL FORM 272 (4-77)
(Formerly NTIS-35)
Department of Commerce
-------
-------