NTID300.5
EFFECTS OF NOISE  ON WILDLIFE  AND OTHER
                  ANIMALS
               DECEMBER 31, 1971
         U.S. Environmental Protection Agency
              Washington, D.C. 20460

-------
                                                                            NTID300.5
        EFFECTS OF NOISE ON WILDLIFE  AND  OTHER
                             ANIMALS
                          DECEMBER 31, 1971
                              Prepared by

                    MEMPHIS STATE UNIVERSITY
                                  under
                        CONTRACT 68-04-0024
                                 for the
                 U.S. Environmental Protection Agency
                Office of Noise Abatement and Control
                         Washington, D.C. 20460
         Thii report has been approved for general availability. The contents of this
         report reflect the views of the contractor, who is responsible for the facts
         and the accuracy of the  data presented herein, and do not necessarily
         reflect the official views or policy of EPA. This report does not constitute
         a standard, specification, or regulation.
For sale by the Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, D.C. 20402 - Price 70

-------
                                                     ii
                   Table of Contents
                                                  Page
Introduction                                         1
Effects of Noise on Laboratory Animals               3
     Effects of Noise on the Auditory System         4
     Non-auditory Effects of Noise                  10
Effects of Noise on Farm Animals                    24
     Effects of Noise on Mammals                    25
     Effects of Noise on Poultry                    28
Demonstrated Effects of Noise on Wildlife           31
     Effects of Noise on Mammals                    32
     Effects of Noise on Birds                      33
     Effects of Noise on Fish                       36
     Effects of Noise on Insects                    37
Suspected Effects of Noise on Wildlife              40
     Interference with Signals                      41
     Direct Effects of Noise                        44
Discussion                                          46
Suggestions for Research                            49
Appendix                                            55
References                                          66

-------
                     INTRODUCTION

       For many years before any real scientific information
was available, we have known that prolonged exposure to
high intensities of noise could cause loss of hearing in
humans.  "Boilermakers" or "artillerymen's" ears have been
known to be defective with the cause of the deficit known
for over a hundred years.  The effects of sound upon man's
hearing are well documented.  In the last few years there
have been studies suggesting a large and potentially
frightening number of non-auditory effects of noise on man;
consequently, today there are many investigators considering
possible non-auditory effects of sound on man and trying to
either demonstrate or disprove them.
       In recent years the possible effects of noise on
wildlife have become a matter of serious concern, for
several excellent reasons.  Our rapidly growing population
and advancing technology result in ever increasing noise
levels.  Noise is an unwanted and at times a potentially
dangerous by-product of virtually every aspect of modern-day
life—construction, transportation, power generation,
manufacturing, recreation, etc.  Today we find that areas
previously considered remote, and therefore relatively
non-polluted by noise, are now being exposed or are in
danger of exposure to various kinds of noise pollution.
The effects that increased noise levels will have on wildlife
in these areas are virtually unknown.  Obviously animals that

-------
 rely on their auditory systems for courtship and mating
 behavior,  prey location,  predator detection, homing,  etc.,
 will be more threatened by increased noise than will species
 that utilize other sensory modalities.   However, due to the
 complex interrelationships that exist among all the organisms
 in an ecosystem,  interference with one  species might well
 affect all the other species.
        In  the past,  man's tampering with the balance  of
 nature frequently has proved to have serious consequences
 for both man and  the ecosystem; whatever affects the
 ecosystem,  eventually affects man also.   Noise pollution
 conceivably could disrupt a balanced ecosystem and  possibly
 even contribute to the extinction of a  vulnerable species.
 Many species of wildlife  today are endangered.   Apart from
 the threat  of  the irretrievable loss of  a particular  species,
 we  have  no  certain knowledge  regarding  the possible effects
 on  our ecology from  such  a loss.   To prevent  possible
 irreparable  damage to  wildlife and to the  balance of  nature,
 it  is mandatory that we calculate  the expected increases
 in  noise levels and  try to relate  them  to  their  possible
 impact on our  wildlife.
       It has  become apparent  that  there  is a  serious  lack
of  information concerning  effects  of  noise on wildlife.
Because of the high  likelihood that  noise effects on  domestic
or  laboratory  animals can  provide clues regarding possible
effects on wild animals, a summary of the literature
concerned with the effects of  noise on non-wild animals is

-------
also included, although it is not as exhaustive nor as
detailed as it would be if that were the mission of this
report.
       For the purpose of this report "wildlife" is defined
as those animals which were not born or hatched in captivity.
The literature search here reported was concentrated on the
period from 1950 to the present, but earlier pertinent
studies are also reviewed.  It was not possible to search
the foreign literature thoroughly in the limited time
available.  Therefore, only clearly relevant and readily
obtainable reports from foreign literature are included.
A detailed report of libraries, information retrieval
services, source materials, and persons and agencies contacted
for information is presented in the Appendix.

        Effects of Noise on Laboratory Animals

       To determine what noise does to an organism, it  is
important to  know:
        (1)  What sounds an animal is exposed to  (e.g.,  frequency,
spectra,  intensity, duration, and pattern of exposure);
        (2)  What factors determine  an animal's susceptibility
to  noise-induced damage  (e.g. species, age, audibility  range,
recovery  process, etc.)  These  factors are best  investigated
in  laboratory experiments using animals, because in laboratory
experiments each of these parameters can be controlled  and
manipulated to determine  the relationships between noise

-------
exposure and effects on the animal.  Experiments investigating
the effects of exposure to noise can be classified in two
basic categories:   (1) studies of effects on the auditory
system, and (2) studies of non-auditory effects.

Effects of Noise on the Auditory System

       As with man, the best documented effect of noise on
laboratory animals is the production of hearing loss or
damage to the auditory system.  This can be produced by a
brief exposure to very loud sound or by prolonged exposure
to moderate levels of sound.
       To study hearing loss it is necessary to measure
hearing abilities before and after exposure to noise.  A major
problem in studying auditory effects of noise on animals is
the determination of what sounds the animal "hears."  Either
electrophysiological recordings from the auditory system, or
behavioral responses of the animal can be used to assess the
sensitivity of the ear.  The Preyer reflex, an ear-twitch
response to sound, indicates that an animal has heard a sound.
This reflex is a reliable, but not a very sensitive test of
hearing in animals, because they are capable of hearing sounds
that are less intense than the sounds that produce the response,
An animal can be trained to respond to a sound stimulus by
using the sound as a cue to obtain reward (e.g., food) or to
escape from punishment (e.g.,  electric shock).  If the animal
is appropriately motivated (i.e.,  hungry or fearful of shock,
depending on the circumstances), his responses can serve as

-------
a very sensitive indicator of what he is hearing.   Auditory
thresholds in animals are frequently determined by using a
"conditioned avoidance response;" the animal is trained to
avoid shock by moving from one side of a two-chambered cage
to the other.  If the animal is well-trained, this procedure
can provide a very sensitive measure of his ability to detect
tones of known frequency and intensity.  An animal's hearing
can be tested, the animal then can be exposed to noise, and
hearing can be retested to determine the decrease in hearing
ability.
       Impulse noise is sound which rises very quickly to its
maximum intensity; it has a very fast rise time, on the order
of a few micro-seconds (i.e., a few millionths of a second).
If sufficiently intense, the rapid pressure changes produced
by impulse sound can damage the ear by rupturing the ear-drum,
by disrupting the chain of tiny bones in the middle ear, or
by damaging the sensory cells and other structures in  the
inner ear.  Poche, Stockwell, and Ades  (1969) studied
histologic changes in 14 young guinea pigs cochleas following
exposure to impulse sound.  Five hundred rounds of paper caps,
producing an average sound-pressure-level  (SPL) of 153 dB,
were fired 30 cm from the ear.  The noises were 1 to 5-sec
apart over a 45-min period.  In 11 of the ears, the sensory
hair cells were destroyed in a narrow band midway along the
organ of Corti.  This damage was comparable  to histologic
changes produced by exposure for 4 hr to a 2,000-Hz tone at
a SPL of 125 to 130 dB.  Majeau-Chargois, Berlin, and  Whitehouse

-------
 (1970)  studied  damage  produced  by  simulated  sonic  booms  in
 24  guinea pigs.  They  determined the  animals' hearing abilities
 by  testing for  the Preyer reflex over a range of frequencies
 from 125  to 16,000 Hz.  The guinea pigs were individually
 exposed to simulated sonic booms having durations  of either
 2.00, 4.76,  or  125.00  milliseconds (each animal was exposed
 to  only one  of  these durations); 1,000 booms were  produced,
 at  a rate  of one per second.  The  intensity  of each boom was
 reported as  approximately 130 dB,  but the reference level was
 not stated.  Tests of  the Preyer reflex following  exposure to
 the booms  failed to detect any  changes in hearing  ability in
 the guinea pigs, although microscopic examinations of their
 cochleas revealed losses of approximately 10% of the hair
 cells in the first turn.  This  was amazingly little damage,
 considering  that each animal was exposed to  1,000  booms  at
 the rate of one per second.
        Because of the very brief durations of impulse sounds,
 they are described in terms of  rise time, maximum  intensity
 (peak pressure level), and duration.    Sounds having a longer
 duration can, in addition, be described by their frequency
spectrum.   A description of the frequency spectrum provides
very useful information because man and other animals are
not equally sensitive to all frequencies.  Sounds  with
different frequency spectra have different effects on the
auditory system.  High frequency pure  tones  or narrow bands
of noise tend to produce changes in localized regions of the
inner ear, whereas low frequency tones, and random or

-------
broad-band noise tend to produce changes throughout the
length of the cochlea.
       In a study of exposure to pure tone, Beagley (1965a,
1965b) exposed 29 guinea pigs to a 500-Hz tone at a SPL of
128 dB.  Following exposure for 20 rain, there was a decrease
in the amplitude of cochlearmicrophonic potentials recorded
from the inner ear, indicating that the ear was less sensitive
to sound.  Also, histological studies revealed extensive
damage to sensory cells and supporting structures in the
third turn of the cochlea, with little or no damage in the
fourth turn.  In studies involving 20 guinea pigs, Conti and
Borgo (1964) found that exposure for 3 hr at a SPL of
100 dB to frequencies of 250, 2,000, 4,000, or 8,000 Hz
produced consistent metabolic changes in the inner ear.
Reduction in the activity of the enzyme cytochrome oxidase
was detectable in several different structures of the inner
ear; this reduction was not related to the frequency of the
stimulating noise.
       Dogs and guinea pigs were used as experimental animals
by Covell (1953) in a study of the histologic changes in
the organ of Corti following exposure to intense sound.  He
exposed 132 guinea pigs and 7 dogs to 50,000 to 100,000-Hz
sound.  Essentially, Covell found marked histologic changes
in the organ of Corti following exposure to intense sound,
indicative of a loss of hearing in the animals.
       In some preliminary studies of temporary threshold
shift (a temporary elevation of the level of lowest intensity

-------
                                                         8

sound that can be heard) in chinchillas, Peters (1965)
determined that temporary threshold shift (TTS) increased
with increased duration of exposure to an octave band of
noise (2,000-4,000 Hz) at 70, 80 or 90 dB (the reference
level was not reported).  In an experiment to determine the
electrophysiological correlates of temporary threshold
shifts, Benitez, Eldredge, and Templer (1970) exposed
chinchillas for 48-72 hr to an octave band of noise centered
at 500 Hz with a SPL of 95 dB.  This exposure produced a
behavioral TTS of about 48 dB in the animals, with recovery
requiring about 5 days.  Changes in cochlear microphonics
recorded from the second turn corresponded closely to
behavioral TTSs; however, losses of sensitivity in activity
recorded from the auditory nerve were much greater than
losses in behavioral responses.  Using an octave band
(300-600 Hz) of thermal noise at a SPL of 100 dB,  Miller,
Rothenberg, and Eldredge (in press) obtained maximum TTSs of
50 dB or more during 7 days of exposure.  Recovery from
these TTSs required about 5 days, with signs of permanent
threshold shifts of less than 10 dB at certain test
frequencies.  Cochlear potentials were reduced and hair
cells were lost in the second and third cochlear turns.
       Broad-band noise has also been used to study hearing
loss and damage to the auditory system.  Lawrence and Yantis
(1957) stimulated guinea pigs with white noise; sound pressure
levels, measured at the tympanic membrane, were 150 dB for
one group of guinea pigs and 136 dB for a second group.

-------
Recordings from the round window indicated that a 20-min
exposure produced some permanent loss in sensitivity in
both groups.  Miller, Watson, and Covell (1963) exposed
37 cats to broad-band noise having nearly equal sound-pressure*
levels across octave bands centered at 850, 1,700, and
3,400 Hz.  Exposures to a SPL of 115 dB for one-eighth of
an hour or 105 dB for one-fourth of an hour produced maximum
TTS at 4,000 Hz.  Exposure to uninterrupted noise at a SPL
of 115 dB for 15 min to 8 hr produced mean permanent
threshold shifts ranging from 5.6 dB (for 15 min) to
40.6 dB (for 8 hr).  Breaking up the total exposure into
small doses resulted in increasingly less permanent loss
as the interval between doses increased; a total of 8 hr of
exposure having 24-hr intervals between sixteen 7 1/2-min
doses produced a permanent threshold shift of only 2 dB.  The
correlation between amounts of permanent threshold shift and
cochlear injury was 0.85.  Ward and Nelson (1970) also
studied the effects of intermittent noise on hearing.  Two
groups of four monaural chinchillas (i.e., animals with one
ear destroyed) were exposed for 2 hr to a 700 to 3,000-Hz
band of noise at a SPL of 117 dB.  One group was exposed
continuously, the other had eight 15-min exposures separated
by intervals of 45 min of quiet.  Both exposures produced
initial threshold shifts of more than 100 dB, but the
animals exposed intermittently had completely recovered
within 2 weeks whereas the animals exposed continuously
had losses of 40 dB 3 months after exposure.

-------
                                                          10

       Twenty guinea pigs were exposed to rocket booster
engine noise by Gonzalez, Miller, and Istre  (1970).  Four
groups of five animals each were  located at  distances from
the noise source of 75, 150, 300, and 5,000  ft respectively.
For the three closer positions, sound-pressure-levels were
above 110 dB from 8 to 8,000 Hz,  with peaks  near 140 dB
between 8 and 31.5 Hz.  Peak pressure levels at the fourth
position were near 110 dB between 16 and 31.5 Hz and dropped
off rapidly in the higher frequencies.  Following 5 min,
50.1 sec of exposure to the rocket engine noise, Preyer
reflex thresholds indicated almost complete  loss of hearing
in the two closer groups, up to 57 days post-exposure; there
were only slight temporary losses in the third group and no
measurable effect in the most distant group.
       Ishii, Takahashi, and Balogh (1969) reported that
exposure for 30 min to white noise at a SPL  of 110 dB
produced reductions in the number of glycogen granules in
guinea pigs' ears.  They suggested that glycogen serves as
an energy source in the hair cells.
       The extent of noise-induced hearing loss or damage to
the auditory system depends upon  intensity, spectrum, duration,
pattern of exposure and individual susceptibility.  Rest
intervals interpolated in exposure periods can significantly
reduce the amount of damage.

Non-auditory Effects of Noise

       Only recently have non-auditory effects of noise

-------
                                                         11

become a matter of concern, due to suggestions that noise
may act as a physiological stressor producing changes similar
to those brought about by exposure to extreme heat, cold,
pain, etc.  There is a considerable body of literature
concerning physiologic response to stress and now there is
also some evidence that exposure to noise may induce similar
changes.  The general pattern of response to stress includes
neural and endocrine activation bringing about a variety of
measurable changes, such as increases in blood pressure,
available glucose, blood levels of corticosteroids, and
changes in the adrenal glands.  There is evidence that
prolonged exposure to severe stress can exhaust an organism's
resources and result in death.  On the other hand, an animal
raised under conditions that protect it from stress becomes
extremely susceptible to disease or even death under even
mildly stressful situations.  The actual significance for
an animal of the physiologic responses to stress is not
understood.
       In an early study, Yeakel, Shenkin, Rothballer, and
McCann (1948) exposed adrenalectomized Norway rats to the
sound of a blast of compressed air 5 min a day, 5 days
a week, for a year.  The average systolic pressure in the
noise exposed rats rose from an initial value of 113 mm Hg
to 154 mm Hg in the last 2 months, while control values rose
from 124 to 127 mm Hg.  More recently (Osinstseva, Pushkina,
Bonashevskaya, and Kaverina, 1969), rats were exposed to an
80 dB noise for various times from 18 to 126 days.  Following

-------
                                                         12
exposure to noise, analyses revealed significant drops in
ascorbic acid contents and weights of the adrenals of these
rats relative to controls.  Adreno-cortical activation has
been studied quite extensively in rodents by Anthony and
Ackerman (1955, 1957) and by Anthony, Ackerman, and Lloyd
(1959).  They exposed rats, mice, and guinea pigs to
relatively broad bands of intense noise:  150-4800 Hz at
140 dB SPL, 10,000-20,000 Hz at 110 dB SPL, or 2,000-40,000 Hz at
132 dB SPL.  Durations of stimulation periods included a single
6-min exposure, 15 min or 45 min per day for up to 12 weeks,
and cycles of 100 min on and 100 min off throughout a
4-week exposure period.  Although they obtained indications
of adrenal activation, as measured by cellular changes in
the adrenal glands and a decrease in the number of circulating
eosinophils, these changes were generally slight and transient.
They did find, however, that intense noise superimposed on
another stress, such a&—restriction of food, could decrease
an animal's life span.  The authors concluded that rats, mice,
and guinea pigs can successfully adapt to noise, but that
noise can have damaging effects if it occurs in conjunction
with additional stressful situations.  They also noted that
intense high frequency noise (132 dB SPL, 2,000-40,000 Hz)
appears to be more stressful than low frequency noise as
evidenced by an increase in noise-induced seizures in
mouse strains considered to be seizure-resistant (Anthony
and Ackerman,  1957).   Jurtshuk, Weltman, and Sackler (1959)
subjected two groups of Wistar albino female rats daily to

-------
                                                         13

1 rain of noise for 11 days and to 5 min of noise for 15
days, respectively.  The noise consisted of 120 Hz at
100  (+5) dB SPL.  Rats that displayed the greatest locomotor
response upon cessation of auditory stimulation also had
lowest blood glutathione levels.  Stimulated rats had
higher adrenal weights and ascorbic acid values and lower
blood glutathione levels than did their controls.  Geber,
Anderson, and Van Dyne (1966) investigated the physiologic
response of rats to three durations of acoustic stress
(15-270 min, 29-96 hr, and 21 days).  The stimulus was a
73 to 93-dB SPL 20,000 to 25,000-Hz sound presented 6 min of
every hour.  They noted lower eosinophil counts, raised
serum cholesterol levels and increased ascorbic acid levels
in the brain.  Although Treptow  (1966) stated that dogs had
transitory increases in glycemic levels in the  blood prior
to becoming used to experimenter handling, he did find a
predictable increase in glycemic reactions in trials 1 and 8
out  of 20 exposures to 80-87 dB  noise for 5-10  min.  Due to
individual reactivities, the measures were highly variable,
but  by trial 20 the dogs had apparently adapted to  the noise
stimulus.
       Biochemical changes due  to  noise exposure were  studied
by Elbowicz-Wariewska  (1962).   Guinea pigs were exposed  for
1 month  to daily 45-min periods  of noise  at  160 £5)  dB SPL
with frequencies from  100  to  50,000 Hz.   Increases  in  lactic
acid dehydrogenase activity and pyruvic  acid levels in the
blood were observed.   Hrubes  (1964) found that  non-esterified

-------
                                                          14
 fatty  acids,  the plasma  lipid most implicated in active
 transport within cells,  increased significantly in female
 white  rats when the rats were exposed to a 95 dB transmitter
 generator noise for 16 hr.  Hrubes and Benes (1965)
 demonstrated  that white rats subjected repeatedly to 95 dB
 noise  developed increased uremic catecholamines, increased
 free fatty acids in blood plasma, and increased suprarenal
 size.  Further, exposed animals showed characteristic weight
 decreases.  Friedman, Byers, and Brown (1967) demonstrated
 that auditory stimulation can interfere with lipid metabolism.
 White  noise at a SPL of 102 dB was presented 24 hr a day
 and an additional intermittent 200 Hz square wave with a
 duration of 1 sec and a SPL of 114 dB was programmed to
 occur  at random intervals, with an average interval of 3 min.
 Thirty rats were exposed to the noise stimuli for 3 weeks
 and 24 rabbits were exposed for 10 weeks.  These animals
 received standard diets and water, but were administered
 additional oils to test their abilities to handle excess fat
while  exposed to noise stress.  Plasma triglycerides were
higher in sound-exposed rats only during the second week;
there were no differences between experimental and control
groups of rats at the end of weeks 1 and 3.  In the rabbits,
however, plasma cholesterol and fasting plasma triglycerides
were higher after 4 weeks of auditory stimulation.  Additional
differences between sound-stressed rabbits and their controls
included deposits of fat in the irises of the eyes of the
experimental rabbits,  plus more aortic atherosclerosis and

-------
                                                         15
higher cholesterol content in their aortas.   The authors
concluded that auditory stress produces changes in handling
exogenously delivered fat, having effects similar to those
produced by chronic hypothalamic stimulation.
       There is additional evidence that sound stimulation
produces its observed effects via cortico-hypothalamic
interactions with the hypophyseal adrenal system.  Werner (1959)
studied the effect of sound on the hypophysis of the rat.  He
found that long, continuous bell ringing (8 hr per day) for
from 1 day to 3 weeks resulted in hypertrophy in the pars
intermedia and hyperactivity in the adrenal cortex.  Ogle and
Lockett (1966) studied the effect in rats of recorded
thunderclaps of 3 to 4-sec duration with a frequency range
of 50-200 Hz at 98-100 dB SPL, presented at a rate of two claps
at 1-min intervals every 5 min for 20 min.  They compared this
effect with that from a pure tone of 150 Hz at 100 dB presented
for 2 min out of every 15 min for 45 min.  Urine was collected
and analyzed for sodium and potassium.  Responses to noise
were analyzed through comparisons among animals that were
intact, that had denervated kidneys and that had neurohypo-
physeal lesions.  The authors concluded that thunderclaps
produced emotional responses which the 150-Hz tone did not
produce.  Thunderclaps affected the hypothalamus resulting
in excretion of oxytocin and vasopressin; these hormones
produced increases in sodium and potassium excretion with
no increase in urine flow.
       In a recent study  (Hiroshige, Sato, Ohta, and Itoh,

-------
                                                          16

 1969), rats were exposed to bell-ringing for 2 min  (spectrum
 and  noise  level were not reported).  Bell-ringing produced
 an increase in the activity of corticotropin-releasing
 factor (CRF) in the hypothalamus.  CRF produces the release
 of adrenocorticotrophic hormone  (ACTH) from the pituitary,
 and  ACTH in turn produces the release of corticosteroids
 from the adrenals.  Monastyrskaya, Prakh'e, and Khaunina
 (1969) reported that sound stimulation produced increases
 in weights of the pituitary and  adrenal glands in healthy
 rats, but not in a strain of sound-sensitive, audiogenic-
 seizure susceptible rats.  The sound-sensitive rats already
 had  enlarged pituitaries and adrenals.  The rats were
 exposed to a 105 dB sound 10 times, for 1.5 min each time,
 with one exposure every 3 to 4 days.  The frequency
 characteristics and noise reference levels were not reported.
 Activity of acetylcholine throughout the rat brain was studied
 by Brzezinska (1968).  Exposure  to noise (type and level not
 reported) for 2 hr a day for 3,  6, 9, 12, or 15 days
 produced gradual increases in acetylcholine esterase activity,
 and an initial increase in acetylcholine concentration
 followed by a decrease with a slow return to normal levels
 by 15 exposures.
       In addition to the pituitary and adrenal glands, the
reproductive glands and functions are also affected by
exposure to noise.   The results are not always consistent,
however.   Anthony and Harclerode (1959) reported negative
results in a study of the effects of noise on sexual scores
of sexually mature male guinea pigs.  Twelve weeks of daily

-------
                                                         17
exposure, for 20 min out of each 30 min period, to noise
at a SPL of 139-144 dB with frequencies of 300 to 4,800 Hz
did not affect the sexual scores of the experimental animals
relative to their controls.  Some evidence of cortico-adrenal
activation was found, however, suggesting that tolerance
limits were approached.  Zoric (1959) exposed 38 male mice
for 8 hr per day for 1-21 days to the sound of an electric
bell.  The level and spectrum of the sound were not reported.
Studies of the testes of sound-exposed mice revealed
involution of the seminal epithelium, partial blockage of
first order spermatocytes, formation of teratocytes, and
atrophy of the epithelium.  He also observed that the
glandular interstitial cells were characterized by hypertrophy
and hyperplasia.  Zondek and Isachar (1964) examined the
effect of acoustic stimulation on genital function in 48
mature rabbits and 3,100 young and mature rats.  The animals
were housed "near" an electric bell 25 cm in diame-ter_ that
rang.l min out of every 10 rain, 24 hr per day, for 9 days
prior to mating.  The peak SPL was 100 dB, with maximum
energy at 4,000 Hz, and another peak of 95 dB at 10,000 Hz.
Auditory stress resulted in enlargement of the ovaries,
persistent estrus, follicle haematomata, and other effects
in female rats and rabbits.  Effects were more pronounced
in female rabbits than in female rats and were hardly visible
in males of either type.  Auditory stress during the
copulatory period induced increased fertility, but during
gestation such stress produced a blockage of pregnancy.

-------
                                                         18

 However,  Zondek (1964)  reported  that  in rats  the males' as
 well  as  the  females'  fertilities were decreased.  The males'
 ability  to fertilize  was reduced to 11% as compared to
 70-80% in control males; comparable effects were produced in
 the female rats.  Sexual behavior did not seem to be
 inhibited (copulation was verified by the presence of a
 vaginal plug),  and there were no changes in the weights of
 the testes and  seminal  vesicles, nor  any noticeable
 anatomical changes in the spermatogenic process.  In similar
 fashion,  Singh  and Rao  (1970) studied the effects of auditory
 stress on rat ovaries.  They exposed  74 adult female rats
 to continuous auditory  stimulation by a 2,000-Hz tone at 100
 dB C  for  up  to  150 days.  They found  that 31 animals developed
 persistent vaginal estrus after 10 consecutive days of stress.
 As the stress was continued, more and more animals demonstrated
 the condition.
       There is evidence that sound stimulation may induce
 lasting changes in exposed animals and even in their offspring,
 at least  in strains of  mice that have been specially bred to
 be susceptible to audiogenic seizures.  Lindzey (1951)
 studied emotionality and audiogenic seizure susceptibility
 in mice exposed to noise.  The animals were stimulated by
 the sound from a bell attached to a metal washtub (spectrum
and SPL were not described).  He reported increased
susceptibility to seizure in certain strains of mice.
Thompson and Sontag (1956) described effects of audiogenic
seizures in pregnant rats on maze-learning abilities of

-------
                                                         19
their offspring.  Each of six male albino rats was bred to
one experimental and one control female.   Two seizures per
day were induced from the fifth through the eighteenth day
of pregnancy in each of the six experimental females.
Within 24 hr of birth two male and two female pups were
selected from each litter and the rest were removed.   Three
mothers in the experimental group and three in the control
group kept their own pups, while the pups of the other
three mothers in each group were switched between groups
so that pups from experimental (seizure) mothers were
cross-fostered on control mothers and vice versa.  At 21
days of age, the pups were removed from the mothers and
housed in individual cages in the animal room.  General
activity levels were tested at 30 and at 60 days of age.
Training in a water maze began at 80 days of age.  Although
there were no significant differences in body weights, litter
sizes, or activity levels, there were significant differences
between experimental and control groups in maze  learning.
Pups born to mothers that had audiogenic seizures during
pregnancy had significantly more errors and required
significantly more trials than did pups born to  controls
even if the control pups were cross-fostered on  experimental
mothers.  Ishii and Yokobori  (1960) found that female mice
exposed to 90,  100, or 110 phon. white noise for 6 hr per
day from the eleventh through the fourteenth day of pregnancy
had more malformed young, more young still-born, and smaller
embryos than did unexposed mice.  Teratogenic effects produced

-------
                                                         20

by audiogenic stress were also reported by Ward, Barletta, and
Kaye  (1970).  A motorcycle horn producing 82-85 dB SPL at
320-580 Hz was timed to deliver noise intermittently for
60-75% of each hour.  Female albino mice (Swiss-Webster strain)
were placed in the chamber and exposed to the noise for at
least 5-hr periods at different stages of pregnancy (vaginal
plug was used as indicant of pregnancy).  The most severe
effects were obtained with stress 8 hr per day on days 8 to
17 of pregnancy.  In these cases, 40% of the litters were
resorbed and mean fetal weight was 0.44 g while mean fetal
weight in control litters was 1.45 g.  Although only moderate
noise levels were used, there were severe results if
stimulation occurred during critical periods.  Stress during
days 7-8 resulted in 100% resorbtion by day 18.  Observed
teratogenic effects (cranial hematoma, dwarfed hind limbs,
and tail defects) were attributed to endocrinologic effects
of stress on the mother and/or the fetus.  These stress
effects resulted in discharge of catecholamines and steroids
from the adrenals.  Decreased uterine and placental blood
flow were considered to be responsible for fetal hypoxia,
and perhaps delayed implantation.  At least one experiment
has shown there is a relation between noise exposure and
susceptibility to viral infection in audiogenic seizure
susceptible strains of mice.  Jensen and Rasmussen (1970)
used an 800-Hz tone with an intensity of 120-123 dB for
3 hr each day on 6-8 week old Swiss Webster BUYS mice.
Mice innoculated intranasally with vesicular stomatitis

-------
                                                         21

virus just before exposure to sound were more susceptible
to the infection, while mice innoculated after the exposure
were more resistent.  The sound stressed mice were also more
susceptible to polyoma virus and developed more tumors than
controls that were not sound-stressed.  The sound suppressed
the progression of Rauscher virus lukemia.  The inflammatory
and interferon responses were also impaired by sound.  They
also found that the sound stressed mice had periods within
each day when they might be more, less or just as susceptible
to viral challenge as non-stressed control subjects.  This
transitory change in susceptibility was found to be independent
of adrenal function.  In addition to undesirable effects of
noise that have been demonstrated in audiogenic-seizure
susceptible mice, a recent study reports noise-induced
hemorrhages in dogs.  Ponomar'kov, Tysik, Kidryavtseva, Barer,
Kostin, Leshchenko, Morozova, Nosokin, and Frolov  (1969)
exposed dogs to 0.6- to 3.5 sec bursts of white noise at
105 to 155 dB.  Two hours after exposure, 3 mm diameter
hemorrhages were found in the lungs, if noise levels exceeded
125 dB.  Increased noise levels resulted in increased numbers
of hemorrhages, but not in increases in the size of each spot.
Emphysematous changes induced by noise exposure were still
detectable at 60 days postexposure, even though hemorrhaged
blood had been resorbed.
       Noise has also been demonstrated to disrupt behavior
in laboratory animals.  Monaenkov  (1958) reported  that rats
exposed for 7 days to sounds produced by electric  bells  (for

-------
                                                          22

 45 rain to 2 hr  per  day)  became  untidy  and  less  active,
 refused to eat,  and became  aggressive.  Borisova  (1960)
 stated that white rats exposed  to  85-dB noise displayed
 weakened conditioned reflexes.  Five days  of rest were
 necessary for the reflexes  to return to normal.
        Permanent effects produced  by raising 80 albino rats
 in two different litter sizes and  under two different sound
 levels were reported by Groh  (1965).  The  rat pups were
 divided into litters of either  3 or 13  animals  then
 randomly assigned to lactating  females  other than their own
 mothers.   Half the  rat pups in  each litter size were raised
 in sound-proof boxes; the other half were  raised in regular
 wire cages in a  noisy animal room.  There  were  10 male and
 10 female  pups in each of the four groups.  After 21 days
 under  these conditions, the rats were weaned and placed,
 four animals to  a cage, in the  common animal room for an
 additional 21 days.  At the end of this period  (42 days)
 measures were made  of body weights, spontaneous activity  in
 an open-field test,  heart rate  increases following electric
 shock, and response  latency in  a straight  runway at the end
 of  20  trials.  Open  field measures were repeated at 56 days
 and body-weights at  57 days.  After these  tests, relative
weights of  the adrenal gland were measured.  Rats in large
 litters weighed  less and had larger adrenal glands.  Rats
raised in  soundproof boxes learned faster  (had  lower latencies)
 in  the straight runway than did rats raised in the animal room.
Decreased  activity in the open  field test  and increased heart

-------
                                                         23
rate responses were more pronounced in rats raised in large
litters in soundproof boxes and in those raised in small
litters in the animal room than were those in the other two
groups.  With the possible exception of the cardiac response,
all these morphological and behavioral changes appeared to
be stable.
       There are several factors which most of the studies
cited above have in common and which merit general comment.
The SPLs used were mostly those which would be described as
high or intense, and the duration of exposure in most cases
was sufficiently short that it would be typified as acute
rather than chronic.  A danger in generalizing from "acute"
high or relatively high intensity level studies to "chronic"
low levels of stimulation is that there may be no relationship
at all.  The longest exposure duration cited in non-auditory
effects was 150 days.  That should probably be considered  a
chronic exposure; however, the next longest exposure was 42
days, which would hardly qualify as a chronic exposure  except
perhaps for relatively short-lived organisms.  The  levels  of
stimulation cited were as high as 160 dB with most  in excess
of 100 dB and with few below 90 dB.  These are levels much
beyond what we would normally find animals exposed  to around
airfields, industries, highways, or other  intrusions by man
into their habitat.  It would seem  logical to expect little
or no auditory  damage to animals from the usual  invasions
by man into the animals* world.  Other  physiological or
endocrinological damage may result, however,  the  evidence

-------
                                                         24

for such damage is at best conflicting and in need of
elaboration.  It would appear that experiments to determine
the effects of long term exposure to lower sound levels
have not been performed.  With respect to non-auditory
effects, it is unlikely that lower levels of stimulation
for moderate durations would produce observable changes in
laboratory animals in sexual function, cholesterol or
ascorbic acid levels, etc.  Another important fact which
should be made explicit here is that the audible range
of hearing varies widely from organism to organism.  This
might be expected to be a significant factor in studies to
determine the effects of sound on the organism.  Little or
no mention of this is found in most of the studies cited,
nor is there any evidence of concern about this factor.
       In summary, in laboratory animals high levels of
stimulation for fairly short durations have produced results
suggestive of significant effects of noise on sexual function,
blood chemistry,  auditory function, seizure susceptibility,
etc.  Extreme caution should be used, however, in generalizing
from results obtained on these animals stimulated at the
levels and durations used, to other animals stimulated at
lower levels for different durations.

            Effects  of  Noise on Farm Animals

        Although  some studies have  been conducted on domestic
 animals of  economic importance,  experimental controls and

-------
                                                         25

adequate response measurement techniques have been lacking.
Since no criteria have been established as far as measurement
and recording of sound stimuli and animal responses to these
stimuli, it is difficult to compare the effects of noise on
one type of domestic animal with effects found in other
domestic animals.

Effects of Noise on Mammals
       Swine exposed to five trials of aircraft sound of
120-135 dB showed no injury to gross anatomy or the organ of
Corti when compared to a control group exposed to ambient
noise levels of 70 dB from an airfield  (Bond, Winchester,
Campbell and Webb, 1963).
       Bond (1963) made extensive tests on the effects of
noise on swine.  During acoustic stress consisting of  15 sec
of 130 dB noise repeated four, eight, or more  times, heart
rate monitored by telemetric equipment  attached  to naive
swine increased significantly from normal heart  rate.  Heart
rate decreased 30 sec after cessation of  the sound stress
but had still not returned to pre-exposure level.  Frequencies
employed were between 300 and 600 Hz.   Bond  (1970) also  found
that although no differences in reactions of nursing sows to
frequencies ranging from 200 to 5,000 Hz  were noted at 100  to
120 dB, a recording of  a squeal of a baby pig at 100  dB
elicited the same response.  The reaction consisted of the
nursing sow rising to her feet and searching for the  sound

-------
                                                         26

 source  followed by indifference.  Baby pigs in the absence
 of  the  dam, exposed to  the same sounds as cited above
 typically reacted by huddling together.  The same investigator
 (Bond,  1970) found that exposure to loud sounds (frequency
 and intensity not specified) caused negligible reactions in
 mating  swine.  Sows and boars appeared indifferent to the
 sounds.  Effects on partuition included heavier piglets at
 birth and a weaning from sows exposed to sound of 120 dB from
 6 AM to 6 PM for three  days before partuition until weaning.
 Bond (1963) found that  pigs exposed to jet and propeller
 aircraft sounds reproduced at 120 to 135 dB daily from 6 AM
 to  6 PM from weaning time or before, until slaughter at 200
 pounds  body weight, showed no differences from pigs unexposed
 to  the  sounds with regard to feed intake, feed utilization
 and rate of gain.  (In  his 1970 review of the literature on
 the  physiology and behavior of farm-raised animals, Bond
 cites Bugard, et al. (1960) in reference to effects of noise
 on  young, castrated, male pigs)  Bugard (1960) found that
 93  dB noise for several days (frequency not specified) resulted
 in aldosteronism and severe retention of water and sodium in
 young,   castrated, male  pigs.  He further stated that
 "alarm  signals" recorded from pigs in the slaughter house
 disturbed the pigs more than mechanically produced sounds.
        Parker and Bayley (1960) reported that milk cow herds
within  3 miles of eight air force bases using jet aircraft,
with 13% of the herds within 1 mile of the end of an active

-------
                                                         27

runway, showed no differences in milk production when
compared to herds which were not exposed to the aircraft
noise.  No differences were found between herds close to
the end of the runway and those farther removed.
       Casady and Lehmann (1966) reported that studies
conducted on herds of milk cows at Edwards Air Force Base
may have been biased in that the animals used had been
exposed to 4-8 sonic booms a day for several years.  Therefore,
even though the intensity of the booms used during testing
was higher than those the cows heard daily, the cattle may
have adapted before the actual testing began.  The investigators
found, over all, few abnormal behavioral reactions in large
animals due to sonic booms.
       Bond (1956) in his review of the literature on sound
stimuli effects on man and lower animals, stated that cows
exposed to exploding paper bags every few seconds  for 2 min
during milking did not give milk while the sound stimuli
were present.  Thirty min following the sound stimulation,
70% of normal milk production occurred.  Bond also cites
Oda (1960) who stated that motorboat noise also produced
a decrease in milk production.  However, calf and  heifer
growth was unaffected by motorboat noise.  Bond (1956) also
reported that observers found a mild reaction in dairy and
beef cattle to only  19 out of 104 sonic booms of 2.6-0.75  Ib
per sq ft.  Milk production was unaffected during  the test
period.  In fact, Bond noted that reactions  to  low subsonic

-------
                                                         28
aircraft noise were more pronounced than were reactions
to sonic booms.  Further, the same reactions were observed
in response to flying paper, strange persons, or other
moving objects.  This observation may indicate that "fright"
reactions occur more strongly when the animal sees rather
than hears the object.

Effects of Noise on Poultry
       Stadelman (1958a) found that when fertilized eggs
from white hens were held 1-7 days after laying and then
subjected to incubation under conditions of sound (over
120 dB) or no sound (under 70 dB), no adverse effects
occurred.  The sound produced inside the incubation boxes
consisted of playbacks of recorded background airfield
noises, and noise from propeller and jet aircraft.  Sound
was present eight out of every 20 min from 8 AM to 8 PM
each day and from 8 PM to 8 AM every third night.  There
were no effects on hatchability of eggs or on the quality
of chicks hatched.
       Eighteen New Hampshire and Plymouth Rock hens were
observed for broodiness for 3 days and then divided into two
groups.  Broodiness is defined as the cessation of egg laying
and the onset of egg incubation.  One group was exposed to
the sound levels mentioned above while incubating 12 hatching
eggs each.   Hens in the other group were given 12 hatching
eggs each but were not exposed to sound.  In the group not

-------
                                                         29

exposed to sound, all eggs were hatched.   In the group
exposed to sound, all except one hen stopped brooding
within 2 hr.  The exceptional hen,  although she remained
broody, hatched only one chick from 12 fertilized eggs
(Stadelman, 1958a).
       Stadelman (1958a) also reported that recorded aircraft
flyover noise at 80 to 115 dB at 300 to 600 Hz played daily
from 8 AM to 8 PM and from 8 PM to 8 AM every third night
for 5 out of 20 min from onset of brooding until chicks were
9 weeks old resulted in no difference in weight gain, feeding
efficiency, meat tenderness or yield, or mortality between
sound exposed and non exposed chicks.  It was, however, noted
that the chicks subjected to the noise were observed and
that the presence of the observers could have rendered the
chicks more adaptable to changing situations than chicks
raised under natural conditions.
       In another experiment by the same investigator
(Stadelman, 1958b) 2,400 crossbred meat chicks were exposed
to the same noise levels as described above.  However, the
chicks were on a different schedule.  The chicks were not
exposed to sound until they were 31 days old, at which time
they were exposed for 5 out of every 20 min for 4 hr.  Chicks
were not exposed to the noise again until they reached 45
days old.  The sound schedule above was then reinitiated,
with a 3 day break due to equipment failure, until  they
reached 10 weeks old.  There was no difference  in weight
gain or feeding efficiency in chicks which were or  were  not

-------
                                                         30

 exposed.   One chick was trampled to death when noise was
 initiated  at 31 days and chicks ran to the end of the
 cage away  from the speaker where the sound level was 20 dB
 less.  The investigators hypothesized that during an actual
 flyover, the sound would not be louder at one end of the
 pens than  the other; therefore, there would be no running
 away from  the sound source.
       Seventy-eight broody broad breasted bronze turkeys
 were exposed to recordings of low flying jet planes at 110
 to 135 dB  for 4 min in the third day of broodiness.  This
 sound treatment typically resulted in a cessation of
 broodiness and a resumption of egg laying in a period of
 time shorter than the time period prior to resumption of
 egg laying in hens whose broodiness was interrupted by
 injections of hormones such as progesterone.  In addition,
 hens injected with progesterone showed a reduction in egg
 production during resumption of egg laying whereas the sound
 treatment  of broody hens produced no decrease in egg laying
 when egg laying was resumed following sound stimulation
 (Jeannoutot and Adams, 1961).
       Embryonic chicks exposed to artificial "peeps" which
 mimicked the "peeps" actually emitted by bobwhite quail
 chicks were speeded up or slowed down as a function of the
 rate of speed at which the peeps were emitted.  Three or more
 peeps per sec were instrumental in causing eggs to hatch
whereas less than 3 peeps per sec did not increase hatchability
 in eggs (Vince, 1966).

-------
                                                         31

       Daily sonic booms with SPLs of 0.75 to 1.25 Ib
per sq ft had no adverse effects on the hatchability of
chicken eggs exposed for 21 days during incubation (Bell,
1970).
       One hundred twenty mink were exposed to simulated
sonic booms with peak overpressure in the housing shed
decreasing from 2.0 Ib per sq ft in the front of the shed
to 0.5 Ib per sq ft in the back of the shed in a smooth
gradient.  A mean boom frequency of 485 Hz was used.  Litter
sizes of boomed mink were larger than those born to
non-boomed mink.  Although the first boom resulted in some
apparently curious emergence from nests, no racing, squealing,
or other evidence of panic was observed.  Autopsies of kits
which died of natural causes disclosed no disorders which
could be traced to booming (Travis, Richardson, Minear, and
Bond, 1968).
       Tests in 1967 in Minnesota showed little or no
response to 6 sonic booms in 10 days with reference to
mink bitch behavior during breeding, birth of kits, or
whelping.  No cannibalistic behavior toward kits or any
other evidence of panic was observed (Bell, 1970).

      Demonstrated Effects of Noise on Wildlife

       Few data are available regarding demonstrated effects
of noise on wildlife and much of what is available lacks
specific information concerning noise intensity, spectrum,
and duration of exposure.

-------
                                                         32

Effects of Noise on Mammals

       Sprock, Howard, and Jacob (1967) subjected caged
wild rats and mice to sounds of varying frequencies
(100-25,000 Hz) and SPL (60-140 dB).  The only effects of
noise were decreased nesting near the sound source and
death at very high intensities.  Recorded rat distress
calls were observed to reduce time spent by rats in the
area of the sound source.
       Confined colonies of wild Norway rats and house mice
were exposed to pulsed ultrasound provided by an ultrasonic
generator for 76 and 81 days respectively (Greaves and Rowe,
1969).  The frequency, intensity, pulse duration, and length
of time between pulses were not reported.  After exposure,
the rodents displayed aversion to the sonic field and did
not re-enter the testing ground.
       Cummings (1971) reported that underwater projections
of recorded killer-whale sounds caused migrating gray whales
to reverse their direction of movement.  Similar recordings
were used by Fish and Vania (1971) to prevent movement of
white whales into an Alaskan river during the time that red
salmon fingerlings were migrating to the ocean.  Pure tone
stimuli at 500 and 2,000 Hz and random noise in the band from
500 to 2,000 Hz were projected with the same intensity and
the same on-off times as the killer whale sounds.  These
sounds also kept the white whales from moving up the river,
but since the whales had previously been exposed to the

-------
                                                         33
killer whale sounds no conclusions could be drawn about the
effectiveness of the tones and the random noise in themselves.
       It has been shown that bats are resistant to jamming
(Griffin, McCue, and Grinnell, 1963).  Apparently they orient
themselves so that noise and signal are received from
different angles.  Signal maksing is greatest when noise
and signal are received from the same direction.  A 60-dB
electric bell rung twice a day from 6 to 7 AM and from 8 to 9 PM
for 7 days resulted in histophysiological changes in the pineal
gland and in the supraoptic nucleus in hibernating bats  (Miline,
Devercerski, and Krstic, 1969).  Hill (1970) reported the use
of high frequency sound produced by 12 adjustable (4,000-
18,000 Hz) dog whistles to drive 500-1000 bats from a
nuclear power station.  According to Crummett  (1970), rabbits,
deer, and some species of birds were repelled by an acoustic
jamming signal  (no details regarding the levels of the
acoustic signal were given) produced by Av-Alarm, a commercially
available noise unit.  This unit produces 2 signals having
frequencies of 2,000 and 4,000 Hz, which are amplitude and
frequency modulated to maximize jamming efficiency relative
to the particular species under observation.

•Effects of Noise on Birds

       Birds were most effectively repelled by high-intensity
(not defined) recordings of the species' own distress calls
(Langowski, Wight, and Jacobson,  1969; Messersmith,  1970;

-------
                                                         34
Wight,  1971).  However, the same investigators reported
rapid adaptation even to species specific distress calls
when presented continuously.  For maximum effectiveness,
intermittent presentation was suggested.
        Pearson, Skon, and Corner (1967) reported that
residents of Denver, Colorado, were successful in dispersing
flocks  of starlings by playing recordings of starling
distress calls for four evenings as the birds arrived at
roosts.  The recordings consisted of repeated cycles of 30
sec of  starling distress calls played for 12 rain.  Partici-
pation  in the dispersal effort of about one half of the
human population in urban roost areas appears to be sufficient
to disperse the birds to outlying areas where they are no
longer  a nuisance.  Habituation to the recordings was not
evident, although some residents played the recordings
continuously.
       Thompson, Grant, Pearson, and Corner (1968a) subjected
groups of starlings to one of five different sounds and found
evidence that the birds perceived specific information through
differential auditory stimulation.   The response measure was
heart rate, telemetrically recorded.  Distress calls produced
by physically restrained starlings were fright producing as
evidenced by a high heart rate acceleration and slow habituation
to the sound.  Escape calls emitted by other starlings
subjected to avian predators caused slight heart rate
acceleration and required two or three applications before

-------
                                                         35
habituation occurred.  A human voice produced elevated
acceleration of heart rate and required two to three
applications before habituation occurred.  Feeding calls
appeared to be "neutral" in that a negligible heart rate
acceleration occurred and habituation took place after an
average of 1.2 applications.  The starlings, it appeared,
were able to discriminate among sound stimuli and react to
them in discrete adaptive ways.
       Thompson, Grant, Pearson, and Corner (1968b) found
that the normal heart rates of wild starlings were elevated
during the day relative to night heart rate values.  The
birds studied were housed individually in acoustical chambers
wherein normal day and night lighting regimes were simulated.
Starling distress calls were used as an acoustical stimulus.
Starlings are normally active during the day, and initial
heart rate responses to 10 sec of the auditory stimulus
during the day were significantly different from baseline
heart rate.  Although the same stimulus produced an initial,
slow increase of heart rate at night, the decrease to baseline
was slower than during the day.  When starlings were tested
individually, the initial response was lower and the decrease
in heart rate faster than when the birds were tested in groups
of five.  Therefore, a "flock effect" seemed to be operating.
       Block (1966) cited the use of tape-recorded distress
calls to disperse roosting starlings during three series of
treatments in 1962.  The number of starlings was reduced from

-------
                                                         36

 10,000 to a few hundred during the experiment.  It was also
 reported, however, that the roosts were subsequently
 reinfested by a majority of the former resident population.
       In the final report of a Committee on the Problem of
 Noise (1963) it was reported that to scare birds a noise
 level of approximately 85 dB SPL at the bird's ear was
 required.  Noise used consisted of loud bangs and birds'
 distress calls.  Birds adapted quickly to the noise and it
 was recommended that in the case of distress calls they be
 used no more than 2 min out of each 20-30 min and only during
 the day.
       A U. S. Department of the Interior report on
 Environmental Impact of the Big Cypress Swamp Jetport (1969)
 discussed B-720 jet flyovers at altitudes of 500 to 5,000
 ft over two sites in the park.  Observers reported that no
 birds were flushed and no disturbances observed.  Noise levels
 ranged from SPLs of 75 dB (with plane at 3,000 ft) to 96.5 dB
 (with plane at 500 ft).  However, it was also reported that
 few birds were in the area at the time and wind effects
 interfered with proper sound level readings.

Effects of Noise on Fish

       The effects of sound on fish have also been studied
 (F A 0 Fisheries Rep. No.  76, 1968).   It was noted in this
report that fishing vessel noise, especially sudden changes
 in noise levels, can scare schooling fish.  Both diving and

-------
                                                         37

changes in direction by fish were observed.   Low frequency
noise appears to be the most frightening type of noise to fish.
       Malar and Kleerekoper (1968) analyzed locomotor
patterns of single naive goldfish before and after exposure
to a 2,000-Hz sound at varying intensities,  30 cm from the
source.  Locomotor patterns of the fish were affected
significantly above an intensity of 2.0 dynes/cm2 (s 80 dB SPL)
       Aplin (1947) reported that underwater explosions for
seismic exploration kill some fish that have air bladders,
especially if they are hit broadside by the pressure wave.
These explosions clearly do not drive fish out of the area
and most species of fish are resistant to these explosions.
       Fitch and Young (1948) also reported fish kills while
using explosives for seismic exploration.  Deaths were caused
primarily by rupture of the air bladders of the fish.  They
also mentioned that on at least three occasions explosions
killed California sea lions, and that occasionally cormorants
were killed while diving and California brown pelicans were
killed if their heads were below the surface during an
explosion.

Effects of Noise on Insects
       The desirability of protecting stored grain from
destruction by insects has led to  several studies directed
at the effects of noise on insects.  Kirkpatrick and  Harein
(1965) reported a 75% reduction  in emerging  Indian-meal  moth

-------
                                                         38

adults following exposure during 4 days of the larval stage
to a  120 to 2,000-Hz sound  (SPL unreported).  Lindgren (1969)
used  a variety of frequencies and intensities to study effects
of sound on Indian-meal moths and flour beetles.  He used
pure  tones of 70 Hz at 110  dB, 200 Hz at 113 dB, 1,700 Hz
at 134 dB, 2,000 Hz at 120  dB, 10,000 Hz at 90 dB, 20,000 Hz
at 71 dB, and 40,000 Hz with SPL not reported.  He also used
variable frequencies of 180-2,000 Hz at 90-105 dB and 180-
2,000 Hz at 90-102 dB.  He  exposed the insects during the
latter part of the pupal stage and for 2 to 4 weeks as unmated
and/or mated adults.  Very  little, if any, effect was noted,
with  the possible exception of mated flour beetles exposed
continuously to 40,000 Hz.  Even though large numbers of
insects were used in many replications, effects of sound
exposure were difficult to  demonstrate, because of variability
in egg production.  The conflict between the data of Kirkpatrick
and Harein (1965) and of Lindgren (1969) possibly can be
explained by stimulation at different stages of the insects'
life  cycles (larval vs. pupal and adult respectively) as well
as by differences in the sound itself.
       Tsao (1969) reported that Indian-meal moths ceased
moving when stimulated by loudspeakers, bells, and whistles.
He noted some evidence of sex-related differences in the
range of 2,000-40,000 Hz.  Cutkomp (1969) reported that a
72-hr exposure to a pulsed sound, having a frequency of
50,000 Hz,  with 25 pulses per sec at 65 dB SPL, reduced
longevity from 20 to 10 days in corn earworm moths and

-------
                                                         39

Mediterranean flour moths.   The sound was an aversive
stimulus in that the insects were observed to move away
from the sound source.   In addition to longevity effects,
the mean number of eggs per female was reduced 59% in the
treated relative to the untreated group.   Arkhepov (1969)
reported that lethal effects of ultrasonic waves occurred
with extensive exposure to high intensities (undefined)
which resulted in thermal and physiochemical changes in
organs and tissues of various animals.
       In a progress report, Shulov (1969) described effects
of pure tones on locusts.  Although tones of 4,000 Hz at
80 dB SPL had little effect on feeding behavior, tones of
1,000, 4,000, and 10,000 Hz elicited a flying response on
more than two out of three trials.
       Honeybees cease moving in response to certain sounds.
Frings and Little (1957) reported that frequencies between
300 and 1,000 Hz with intensities ranging from  107 to  119 dB SPL
produced cessation of movement for up to 20 min.  No habituation
was observed although the study was continued for 2 months.
Experiments by Little  (1959) demonstrated that  stimulation
with sounds having frequencies from 200 to 2,000 Hz produced
cessation of movement in honeybees.  Vibration  of antennae
did not produce the effect, but vibration of any of the
three pairs of legs produced the "freezing response."
       Frings and Frings  (1959) found that certain sounds
attracted swarms of male midges.  Frequencies of  125 Hz  at
13-18 dB above the ambient  noise  level produced agitated

-------
                                                         40

circling of the insects with aggregation around the sound
source.
       The above studies of wildlife show that intense
sound is an aversive stimulus for most organisms studied.
Sound, under somewhat longer exposure conditions, appears
capable of inducing measurable physiological and behavioral
changes in some organisms.  Commercial use is now being made
of acoustic devices to repel certain undesirable animals;
it is logical to assume sound may also repel desirable
animals as well.  Insects also seem to be significantly
influenced by sound, something to consider because insects
are important items in many animals1 diets and significant
links in the food chain.  Apparently an insect's life span
and reproductive capacity may be affected by exposure to
certain sounds.  These findings certainly suggest caution
should be exercised in allowing sound intrusion into animal
habitats, not only because of possible direct effects on the
animals themselves but also on items in the food chain of
the animal.

        Suspected Effects of Noise on Wildlife

       Although there is a limited body of literature dealing
directly with the effects of noise on wildlife, possible
effects can be inferred from information dealing with:
(1) signal production and communication; (2) auditory ranges
for different species; (3) direct effects of noise that have

-------
                                                         41

been demonstrated in laboratory or domestic animals,  and
(4) incidental observations of responses to noise in wild
animals.  The suspected effects can be categorized as either
interference with signals or direct effects on the animal.

Interference with Signals
       Thorpe (1969) discusses the significance of bird
vocalizations and reports that the various calls convey
many types of information such as distress, danger or alarm,
warnings about territorial boundaries, recognition of a mate
or of young, and presence of food.  Increases in background
noise can mask these signals and thus potentially influence
such processes as spacing to obtain optimum population
densities in an area, nesting and care of young, and
detection of prey or escape from a predator.
       Dooling, Mulligan, and Miller  (in press)  reported
that the common canary has its greatest auditory sensitivity
to the range of frequencies from  2,000  to  4,000  Hz, which is
also the range of frequencies maximally represented  in  its
songs.  If  this finding  is representative,  it would  permit
prediction  of which species would be  most  likely to  be  affected
by a noise  having defined frequency characteristics.  They
discussed the relative  importance of  range of sensitivity,
thresholds,  frequency  discrimination, and  sound localization
and concluded that  the  auditory capacity  that is most essential
to the  organism will have the greatest representation in the

-------
                                                         42

 auditory system, at the expense of the others.  That is, a
 capacity such as hearing sensitivity would be greatest in
 animals that rely heavily on auditory signals to survive
 (e.g., nocturnal predators and nocturnal prey) whereas
 frequency resolution would be more important to an animal
 that utilizes intra-specific signals to recognize and call
 a mate or to stake out a territory.
       Potash (in press) reported that male Japanese quail,
 isolated from their mates, increased the frequency of their
 "separation calls" when ambient noise levels were increased
 from 36 dB A to 63 dB A.  The increase in the frequency
 of the calls improved the signal to noise ratio.  Such an
 increase should make detection and recognition of the signal
 and localization of the caller more likely.  The ultimate
 significance to the quail is determined by whether the mate
 responds to the "separation call" before a predator does.
       In attempting to analyze possible signal-masking
 effects of noise on animals, it is important to remember
 that different species are able to detect "sounds" that man
 cannot hear (e.g., the dog's response to the "silent" dog
whistle).   Sewell (1970) reported that rodents both emit and
respond to ultrasonic frequencies ranging up to 40,000 Hz
or even to 70,000 or 80,000 Hz in special cases.  Pye (1970)
reported the production of ultrasonic (i.e., above 20,000 Hz)
signals by certain grasshoppers and moths, as well as from
many kinds of rodents and bats.  However, the audible range
of most birds and reptiles lies well within man's audible
range (Konishi,  1970;  Manley, 1970).

-------
                                                         43

       Interference with signals has sometimes been used by
man in attempts to control unwanted species.   A commercially
available device that broadcasts an acoustic  jamming signal
was described by Crummett (1970).  The signal consisted of
two different frequencies, at about 2,000 Hz  and 4,000 Hz,
which were frequency and amplitude modulated to provide a
signal said to be compatible with species' specific neural
time constants, thus maximizing jamming efficiency and
minimizing adaptation.  In a progress report, Messersmith
(1970) described results of tests using acoustic signals to
control crop depredations by birds.  A commercially available
device was used on blackbird flocks feeding on grain and
recordings of starling distress calls were used on starlings
feeding on grapes.  Both "...were temporarily effective when
used at high volumes and aimed directly at the birds."  Diehl
(1969) reported that a 22,000 Hz sound prevented new
populations of rodents from entering the  area protected by
the sound, although it was necessary to remove resident
populations of rodents by trapping or poisoning.   It  is
possible that similar signal interference effects were
produced by the "hum" of power  lines which were reported  to
disturb reindeer and to contribute to difficulties  in
herding  (Klein, 1971).  The use  of recorded  distress  calls
also represents attempts to interfere with signals, and
thus control certain unwanted species  (e.g.,  Block, 1966;
Fitzwater, 1970; Frings and Frings,  1957; Frings and  Jumber,
1954; Pearson, Skon, and Corner,  1967).

-------
                                                         44
Direct  Effects  of Noise

        It  is very possible that many of the noise-induced
physiological and behavioral changes that have been
demonstrated in laboratory animals could also occur in
wild animals.   Of course, it is very unlikely that wild
animals will be subjected to noises intense enough or of
sufficient duration to produce permanent hearing  losses.
However, chronic exposure to moderate noise levels could
produce some hearing loss or influence processes  that are
hormonally regulated due to noise-induced stress  responses.
Until studies are performed in which effects due  to exposure
to noise are separated from effects due to capture, handling,
or other kinds  of interference, these answers will not be
known.
       Sonic booms, and especially the threat of  the SST's
"super-boom," generated extensive speculation about their
effects on animals.  Davis (1967) described his observations
of some ravens  in Wales.  When the boom occurred, three or
four ravens that had been cruising in the area were rapidly
joined by others.  Within 5 min approximately 70 ravens
were agitatedly circling; 30 min later about 30 ravens were
still flying in the area.  Shaw (1970) reported that adult
condors were very sensitive to noise and abandoned their
nests when disturbed by blasting,  sonic booms or even
traffic noise.   The most deleterious effects attributed to
sonic booms were recent mass hatching failures of sooty terns

-------
                                                         45
in Dry Tortugas, Florida, discussed by Bell  (1970) and
Henkin  (1969).  Following 50 years of breeding success,
99% of the terns' eggs failed to hatch in 1969.  Extremely
low-altitude supersonic flights over the area may have
driven birds off their nests and damaged the uncovered eggs.
Graham  (1969) reported observations of destruction of pelican
eggs by gulls when white pelicans were driven off their nests
by sonic booms.  Graham also said that a fisherman described
the reaction of fish to sonic boom as "similar to those
dynamited in a fishpond."   (Author's Note:  with the impedance
mismatch between air and water this would seem an obvious
impossibility and appear to lend credence to allegations
made regarding the veracity of fishermen).  Bell (1970), in
a recent review of animals' responses to sonic booms, described
only minimal reactions to sonic booms among domestic animals,
ranch mink, and wild animals.  The only clearly detrimental
effect that he discussed was the Dry Tortugas sooty terns'
hatching failure.  A startle response to a sonic boom was
the typical reaction that he reported.
       Clearly, the animals that will be directly affected
by nQise are those that are capable of responding to sound
energy, and especially the animals that rely on auditory
signals to find mates, stake out territories, recognize
young, detect and locate prey, and evade predators.  These
functions could be critically affected even if the animals
appear to be completely adapted to the noise (i.e., they

-------
                                                         46
 show  no  behavioral response such as startle or avoidance).
 Ultimately  it  does not matter  to the animal whether these
 vital processes are affected through signal-masking, hearing
 loss, or effects on the neuro-endocrine system.  Even though
 only  those  animals capable of  responding to sound could be
 directly affected by noise, competition for food and space
 in an ecological niche appropriate to an animal's needs,
 results  in  complex interrelationships among all the animals
 in an ecosystem.  Consequently, even animals that are not
 responsive  to  or do not rely on sound signals for important
 functions could be indirectly  affected when noise affects
 animals  at  some other point in the ecosystem.  The "balance of
 nature"  can be disrupted by disturbing this balance at even
 one point.  We would do well to have some knowledge of what
 to expect from noise pollution in wildlife habitats before
 it produces its effects.

                      Discussion

       It is now time for an overview of the literature
found and a discussion of what it might mean.  The best
documented, most clearly proven effect of high intensity
noise exposure on hearing organisms is that of damage to
the auditory structure with a resulting loss of hearing.
Now,  assuming that the levels of noise produced are
sufficient in an area to produce a loss of hearing in a given
animal,  what are the likely or possible consequences of

-------
                                                         47
such a decrease in auditory sensitivity?  First, it should
be noted that animals differ in their audible range and the
audible range for all animals is not known.  Anticipated
consequences of a loss of hearing ability are many.  The
prey-predator situation could be drastically changed.  The
animal that depends on its ears to locate prey could
starve if auditory acuity decreased, the animal that
depends on hearing to detect and avoid its predators could
be killed.  Reception of auditory mating signals could be
diminished and affect reproduction.  (Masking of these
signals by noise in an area could also produce the same
effect).  Detection of cries of the young by the mother
could be hindered, leading to increased rates of infant
mortality or decreased survival rates.  Distress or warning
calls may not be received, again significantly affecting
survival.
       Considerably less assurance is possible in discussing
the likely consequences of non-auditory effects.  For one
thing, at best some of the effects are small, many are not
clear cut and reproducible under precisely controlled
conditions, and some are only suggested.  But assuming that
there are non-auditory effects, as reported, an attempt will
be made to anticipate some of their consequences.
       The reports of significant changes in.reproductive
organs  (testes and ovaries) and sexual function  (estrus)
should be viewed as possible serious threats to the  animal's

-------
                                                         48

reproductive capacity.  If chronic exposure to sound pressure
levels expected to result from known or projected sound
sources could be shown to produce such effects, there can
be little doubt about the danger to the species.  Studies
to verify and elaborate such effects should be made as soon
as possible.
       The literature describing audiogenic seizures
following noise exposure, and possibly demonstrating
increased susceptibility to audiogenic seizures in fetuses
exposed to sound during critical stages of pregnancy can
almost be dismissed summarily.  First, audiogenic seizures
can be induced in only certain strains of animals of a
particular species.  It is exceedingly difficult to induce
seizures to acoustic stimuli in animals other than genetic
strains known to be susceptible.  There are references to
such seizures in isolated individuals of various species
including man but they are apparently rare.  Thus we
dismiss this effect as one meriting little or no further
concern.
       A number of physiological measures have revealed
noise-induced changes in a variety of animal species.
Apparently noise can affect the hypothalamic-hypophyseal
system, producing alterations in electrolyte excretion,
circulating blood levels of eosinophils, and release of
catecholamines and steroids from the adrenals.  Such
changes can affect animals' abilities to withstand additional
stress, and influence such hormonally-regulated functions as

-------
                                                         49

mating and reproduction.   Obviously these effects could
have serious consequences for the species as well as for
the individual organism.   Sophisticated equipment and
techniques will be necessary to obtain measures of such
changes in wild animals in their natural habitat, if at all
possible, so that noise-induced changes will not be
confounded or masked by changes due to captivity and restraint,
       The possible consequences of some of the behavioral
effects noted are difficult to evaluate.  Decreased
exploratory behavior, immobility, and things of like nature
could have significant consequences if they occur under
conditions of chronic stimulation and do not adapt out
over time.  Any panic type behavior such as piling up or
huddling, could well lead to problems for survival of an
animal.  Also, avoidance behavior could restrict access to
food or shelter and therefore adversely affect an animal's
or even a species' chances for survival.
       In general then, few if any of the reported or
suggested effects of noise on animals would benefit the
animal or increase his chances for survival.  On the other
hand, some of them might possibly lead to his death or
decrease his chances of survival.

               Suggestions for Research

       In examining the literature on the effects of noise
on animals in general and on wildlife in particular, it  is

-------
                                                         50

 extremely  difficult  to  find where  to begin in detailing
 needs  for  research.  There are at  least two reasons for
 this.   With  the  exception of the large, well done body of
 literature exploring the effects of noise upon auditory
 structures and hearing, well controlled, well designed
 experiments  substantiating non-auditory effects of noise
 are rare.  In the case  of wildlife, such studies are
 virtually  nonexistent.
        It  is apparent then, that at least two different
 concomitant  programs of research are indicated in order to
 fill the large gaps  in  our scientific understanding of the
 nature  and extent of the effects of noise upon wildlife.
 A thorough,  meticulous, and precise program systematically
 studying the effects of long term  low level "chronic" noise
 exposure should be initiated to eliminate the uncertainties,
 ambiguities, and even conflicts in reports of non-auditory
 physiological, metabolic, sexual,  and other physical effects
 of noise.  It could  well be that effects noted with "acute"
 exposure might not be observed under conditions of "chronic"
 exposure.  It should not be necessary to add that the
 intensity,  spectrum, and duration  of exposure should be
precisely set and controlled.   Such a program should consider
the auditory sensitivity of the specific animal studied and
tailor acoustic stimulation to maximize the likelihood of
results.
       Concurrent with careful examination of physiological
and other physical and chemical effects of noise on animals

-------
                                                         51

should be a program of research devoted to the study of
effects of noise on true wildlife,  existing in their native
habitat under normal conditions.  Such a program would have
many aspects and would of necessity require a multi-disciplinary
approach.  An adequate approach to the problem would entail
study of many factors.  Census counts of animals in their
natural habitat would be necessary as well as detailed
studies of their normal blood chemistry, reproductive
functions, and any other aspects that there is reason to
believe may be affected by changes in ambient noise exposure
levels.  A survey of the habitat should be made in depth,
i.e., over long enough periods of time so that sufficient
knowledge is amassed regarding infrequently occurring but
relevant events.  Once sufficient knowledge is available
about the environment and its inhabitants, the sound levels
in the environment should be systematically varied and the
effects of such changes on the population compared with
pre-change data for all of the  levels considered.  The
changes in level, for the sake  of validity, could well be
due to sound one might expect from technological advances,
i.e., aircraft noise, other transportation noise, or industrial
noise.  Such a course would at  least provide  face validity
for the results.  Such changes  in level should be maintained
for a considerable length of time to provide  "chronic" rather
than "acute" data.  A minimum time course for a study  of this
nature, in the field, and under the conditions outlined above,
would be 3 to 4 years.  For some types  of animals in some

-------
                                                         52

places more time would be needed.  At the same time in field
studies, efforts should be made to determine whether the
animals leave the area upon stimulation by higher levels
of sound, and if so, do they later return, or is their
place taken by other animals of the same, or other species.
Other relevant questions to be answered would include, does
the animal density level in the area increase, decrease, or
remain the same?  Does the general health, weight, etc. of
the animal change?  A study of predator-prey relations might
also be valuable, to determine possible noise related but
unobvious causes for changes in the population.  Certainly
the food supply of animals is important and if the data
suggesting noise effects on insects were correct, the food
source of some of the other animals in an area could change
and thus be responsible for subsequent changes in the animal
population.  An essential part of a research program such as
that suggested above would be to provide a control study area
contiguous to the experimental areas and as similar as
possible in every way.  This kind of design is mandatory
because of the wide normal variations in the population
density of a great many animals.  If unaccounted for, these
cyclic normal fluctuations in animal populations might
completely mask any real effects, if any, from the noise.
       An important consideration in planning research should
be the frequencies to be investigated, as well as the sound
levels.  Frequencies that are inaudible to humans (ultrasound)
are well within the audible range of many animal species.

-------
                                                         53

Aquatic mammals, bats, and rodents, emit cries having very
high frequency components, which are considered to play
an important role in communication.  Potential noise sources
must be analyzed to determine what ultrasonic (to humans)
as well as what audible frequencies will be produced, and
the impact of the entire range of expected frequencies on
wildlife must be investigated.
       Another area where research effort would seem to be
justified and indicated would be that of effects of noise
on various domestic animals.  There are clear suggestions
of possible influences of noise on sexual function, on the
fetus and mother during pregnancy, on weight gain and
utilization of food.  In view of the economic importance of
cattle, chickens, turkeys, sheep, and the many other domestic
animals it is clear that research in this area might prove of
value.  Research of this general type is currently underway
at the Institute National Reserche Agronomique, Jouy En  Josas,
France (personal communication, Dr. R. G. Busnel, INRA).
For example, a problem they are currently considering is
how to deafen young chickens cheaply and safely.  They have
evidence which leads them to suspect deaf chickens might
gain more weight from the same amount of feed, presumably
because they were less distracted by the noises of the
other chickens around them, were less nervous, or perhaps
had lower activity levels.
       It is exceedingly difficult to assign priorities  to
the research suggested above.  When all of  it  is  necessary

-------
                                                         54
and should be done in order to provide the complete
information essential for decisions, all that can be done
by way of assigning priorities is to point out that possibly
more information of immediate and practical use could be
gleaned from field studies than from laboratory studies.  If
conducted on a sufficiently large scale and encompassing a
large enough scope, vital information regarding the effects
of noise on wildlife could be secured in 3 or 4 years.  It
would still be required that concurrent laboratory studies
be conducted, however, in order to obtain information that
could only be secured through laboratory research.

-------
                                                         55

                       Appendix*
       The literature search can generally be divided into
a search by manual, computer, personal interview, and
written communication means.  To assure depth of coverage,
the literature of medicine, agriculture, conservation,
and science was searched.
       Manual searches were conducted in the public catalogs
of Memphis State University Library (John Brister Library),
University of Tennessee Medical Library (Mooney Memorial
Library), and other libraries listed in the source biblio-
graphy.  A relatively small number of books and monographs
was found in the catalogs.  A comprehensive manual search
on the abstracts, indexes, and bibliographies listed  in
the source bibliography was carried out.
       It is the desire of everyone who has worked on this
search to thank the many people who helped in any way,
especially those who gave  time for personal  interviews and
correspondence.
*The literature search was conducted under Contract No.  68-04-0024
from the Environmental Protection Agency under  the direction of
Dr. John L. Fletcher, Professor of Psychology,  and Dr. Michael J.
Harvey, Associate Professor of Biology.  Wilma  P. Hendrix
compiled the source bibliography and served  as  library consultant.
The information was obtained  and analyzed by June W.  Blackwell,
Virginia M. Norton, Clara B.  Davis, and Richard L. Taylor.

-------
                                                         56


               Library Catalogs Searched


Department of the Interior Library

H. W. Calhoun Medical Library (Administrative Headquarters
       for the Southeastern Regional Medical Program)

John Brister Library, Memphis State University

Library of Congress

Library of the National Academy of Science

Mooney Memorial Library, University of Tennessee Medical Units

National Library of Medicine

Smithsonian Institution, Library of Natural History

Robert W. Woodruff, Library for Advanced Studies, Emory
       University



                   Computer Searches


Alabama MEDLARS Center
The University of Alabama
Medical Center Library
Birmingham, Alabama 35233

     Effects of Noise Pollution on Wildlife, January, 1964 -
          December, 1968.

          Key Words:  Animal kingdom - invertebrates
                      Animal kingdom - vertebrates
                      Acoustic trauma
                      Acoustics
                      Audiometry
                      Auditory perception
                      Auditory threshold
                      Hearing
                      Hearing tests
                      Noise
                      Pitch discrimination
                      Sound
                      Ultrasonics

     Effects of Sound on Wildlife,  January, 1969 - July, 1971.

          Key Words:  Animal kingdom - invertebrates

-------
                                                         57
Computer Searches (continued).

                      Animal kingdom - vertebrates
                      Acoustic trauma
                      Acoustics
                      Auditory perception
                      Hearing
                      Hearing tests
                      Noise
                      Sound

Library Reference Service, Current and on going research
Conservation Library Center
Federal Aid in Fish and Wildlife
Denver Public Library
1357 Broadway
Denver, Colorado 80203

     Noise Pollution and its Effects on Wildlife
North Carolina Science and Technology Research Center (STRC)
Research Triangle Park, North Carolina 27709

     Biological Abstracts, 1959 - June 1971

          Key words taken from B.A.S.I.C. Keyword and
             Subject  Index

     Preliminary searches were conducted on each of the
     following:

          The NASA  Information File
          Department  of Defense File
          Engineering Index
          Chemical  Abstracts

     The results of the preliminary searches  were such that
     the STRC engineers advised that  no further attempts  be
     made to search these files for materials on noise and
     its effects on wildlife
 Science Information Exchange
 Smithsonian Institution
 A National Registry of  Research in Progress
 Madison National Bank Building
 1730 M. Street,  N.  W.
 Washington, D.  C.  20036

      Effects of Noise,  Ultrasonics, and Other Sound
          Frequencies on Wildlife and Insects

-------
                                                         58
Computer Searches  (continued).

     Aircraft Noise and Sonic Boom Studies:  Effects on
        People, Animals and Buildings
     Generation and Propagation of Noise
     Development and Design of Low Noise Aircraft Engines
     Noise in the Vicinity of Airports
     Noise Abatement Studies
       Abstracts, Indexes, and Catalogs Searched


Abstracts of World Medicine.  London, British Medical
       Association, 1947 - May 1971.

Agricultural Index.  New York, H. W. Wilson, 1950 -
       1964.  (Ceased publication)

Armed Forces Medical Library Catalog.  U. S. Army Medical
       Library, 1950-54.

Bibliographic Index.  New York, H. W. Wilson, 1950 -
       June 1971.

Bibliography of Agriculture.  Department of Agriculture.
       Washington, D. C., 1942 - June 1971.

Biological Abstracts.  Philadelphia, Pa., 1950-1971.

Biological and Agricultural Index.  New York, H. W. Wilson,
       1964 - June 1971.

Bioresearch Titles.  Philadelphia, Pa., Bioscience Information
       Service of Biological Abstracts, 1965 -  1967.

Bioresearch Index.  Philadelphia, Pa., Bioscience Information
       Service of Biological Abstracts, 1967 -  May 1971.

Books in Print.  New York, Bowker, 1970-71.  (One year)

British Abstracts of Medical Science.  London,  Pergamon Press,
       for Biological and Medical Abstracts, 1954 - 1956.

Catalog of Grants.  Washington, D. C., National Science
       Foundation, 1970 - June 1971.

Cumulative Book Index.  New York, H. W. Wilson, 1950 -
       June 1971

Cumulative Veterinary Index; a selected list of publications
       from the American literature.  Arvada, Colorado,
       Index Incorporated, 1970 - May 1971.

-------
                                                         59
Current Contents - Life Sciences.  Philadelphia, Institute
       for Scientific Information, 1958 - June 9, 1971

DSH Abstracts.  American Speech and Hearing Association and
       Gallaudet College.  Washington, Deafness, Speech,
       and Hearing Publisher, 1960 - June, 1971.

Dissertation Abstracts.  Ann Arbor, Michigan, University
       Microfilms, 1952 - June, 1971.

Environmental Lav Abstracts.  Oak Ridge, Tenn., Oak Ridge
       National Laboratory, 1955 - February, 1971.

Excerpta Medica, Herengracht, Amsterdam.

       Oto-, Rhino-, Laryngology,
            Section XI, Vol. 1 (1948) - Vol. 24, No. 6
              (June, 1971)

       General Pathology and Pathological Anatomy,
            Section V, VoTT"! (1948; - vol. 24, No. 4
              (April, 1971)

       Public Health, Social Medicine, and Hygiene,
            Section XVII, Vol. 1  (1955) - VolT  17, No. 4
              (April, 1971)

Index Catalogue of the Library of the Surgeon General's
       Office, United States Army.  Washington, Superintendent
       of Documents.  Series, 1880-1961.

Index Medicus

       Quarterly Cumulative Index to Current Medical
          Literature.  AMA, Chicago,  1916-26.

       Quarterly Cumulative Index Medicus.  AMA, Chicago,
          1927-1956.

       Current List of Medical Literature.  AMA, Chicago,
          1950-1959.

       Current List of Medical Literature.  Army Medical
          Library, Washington, (Vols. 19-36), 1950-1959.

       Index Medicus.  American Medical Association,
          Chicago, 1960 - July 1971.

International Abstracts of Biological Sciences.  London,
       Pergamon Press, 1956 - May 1970.

Monthly Catalog.  Washington, Superintendent of Documents,
       1950 - May 1970.

-------
                                                          60
National Library of Medicine Catalog.  Washington, D. C.,
       National Institute of Health, Public Health Service,
       Health, Education and Welfare Department,  1955 -  1965.

National Library of Medicine Current Catalog.  Washington,
       D. C., National  Institute of Health, Public Health
       Service, Health, Education and Welfare Department,
       1966 - 1970.

Pandex.  New York, Pandex, Inc., 1967 -  1968  (Microfiche)
       (Published since 1969 by CCM Information Science
        Incorporated, New York)

Pollution Abstracts.  W. Farmer.  La Jolla, California,
       1970 - June 1971.

Psychological Abstracts.  American Psychological Association,
       Incorporated, Washington, D. C.,  1950 - June  1971.

Public Affairs Information Service.  Bulletin of  the Public
       Affairs Information Service.  New York, 1950  -
       December 1970.

Readers' Guide to Periodical Literature.  New York,  H. W.
       Wilson, 1950 - May 1971.

Science Citation Index.  Philadelphia, Institute  for
       Scientific. Information, 1961 - May 1971.

U. S. Government Research Reports.  Department of Commerce.
       Clearing House for Federal Scientific and Technical
       Information, Washington, D. C.:

       U. S. Government Research Reports, 1954 -  1964.

       Government-wide Index to Federal  Research and
          Development Reports, 1965 - 1970.

       U. S. Government Research and Development Reports,
          January 1971 - May 1971.

       Government Reports Index, June 1971. (one issue,  name
         of publication changed)

Wildlife Abstracts.  Washington, D. C.,  Fish and Wildlife
       Service,  1954 - December 1970.

Zoological Record.   London.   The Zoological Society of
       London, 1950 - May 1971.

-------
                                                         61


                Bibliographies Searched


Acoustical Society of America.  Report of the 80th Annual
       Meeting.  November, 1970, Houston, Texas.

Advances in Ecological Research.  New York, Academic Press,
       1962 - June 1971.

Advances in Environmental Sciences.  New York, Wiley -
       Interscience, Vol. 1 - 1969 - June 1971.

Environment.  St. Louis, Missouri, Committee for Environmental
       Information.  Vol. 1, No. 1, January-February, 1961.

Environmental Research.  New York, Academic Press, Inc.,
       Vol. 1, No. 1, June, 1967 - June  1971.

Environmental Science and Technology.  Washington, D. C.,
       American Chemical Society Publications, Vol.  1,
       No. 1, January 1967 - June  1971.

Heinemann, Jack M.  Effects of  Sonic Booms on  the Hatchability
       of Chicken Eggs and Other Studies of Aircraft-Generated
       Noise Effects on Animals.  TRW  Life Sciences  Center.
       Hazleton Laboratories, Inc., 1965.

International Civil Aviation Organization.  Sonic Boom  Panel,
       Supplement.  Montreal, 12-21 October,  1970.
       pp. 1-55/1-59.  Doc. 8894,  SBP/II.

National Academy of Science.  National Research Council.
       Committee on SST - Sonic Boom,  Subcommittee  on
       Animal Response.  An Annotated  Bibliography  on
       Animal Response  to Sonic Booms  and  Other Loud
       Sounds.  Washington, D.  C.,  1970.

Rice, C. G. and G. M. Lilley.   University of Southampton.
       Report  in five parts on  the sonic boom.  Prepared for
       the OECD Conference on Sonic Boom Research.   Part 4,  1969,

Science and Citizen.  St. Louis,  Missouri, Committee for
       Environmental  Information,  Vol. I-X,  1958 - 1968.

United Nations.  Food and Agricultural Organization of  the
       United  Nations.  Report  on a Meeting  for Consultations
       On Underwater  Noise, Rome,  Italy, December,  1968.  (1970)

U.  S. Department of Health, Education and  Welfare,  Public
       Health  Section.  Reports on the Epidemology and
       Surveillance of  Injuries.   No.  FY 71-RI.  The Role
       of Noise  as a  Physiologic  Stressor.  pp. 1-59, 1969.

-------
                                                         62
             Persons Providing Materials,

             Information, and Assistance


Bible, Senator Alan.   (Nevada), Chairman of the Subcommittee
       on Parks and Recreation Hearings on Alterrain Vehicles
       on Public Lands.

Bond, James.  Research Animal Scientist, Animal Science
       Research Service, U. S. Department of Agriculture,
       BeItsvilie, Maryland.

Carlisle, John G., Jr.  Associate Marine Biologist, Department
       of Fish and Game, Marine Resources Region, 350 Golden
       Shore, Long Beach, California 90802.

Chatham, George N.  Analyst in Environmental Policy, Environ-
       mental Policy Division, Legislative Reference Service,
       Library of Congress.

Cope, Oliver.  Fisheries Research, Bureau of Sport Fisheries
       and Wildlife, Fish and Wildlife Service, Department
       of the Interior, Washington, D. C.

Crummett, James G.  Av-Alarm Corporation, 960 N. San Antonio Rd.,
       Suite 170, Los Altos, California 94022

Curtis, William H.  The Wilderness Society, Washington, D. C.

Fish, James F.   Naval Undersea Research and Development Center,
       Department of the Navy, San Diego, California 92132.

Foster, Charles R.  Department of Transportation, 400 7th Street,
       S. W., Washington, D. C.

Gales, Robert S.  Naval Undersea Research and Development
       Center (The Listening Group), San Diego, California 92132.

Gates, Doyle.  Manager, Marine Resources Region, California
       Department of Fish and Game, 350 Golden Shore,
       Long Beach, California 90802.

Konishi, Masakazu.  Associate Professor, Department of Biology,
       Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08540.

Lemke, Darrell H.  Coordinator of Library Programs, Consortium
       of Universities, Washington, D. C.

Lipscomb, David M.   Associate Professor of Audiology and
       Speech Pathology, Director, University of Tennessee
       Noise Study Laboratory, Knoxville, Tennessee 37916.

-------
                                                         63
Marler, Peter.  Professor of Animal Behavior,  Department of
       Animal Behavior, Rockefeller University,  66th Street
       and York Avenue, New York, New York 10021.

Miller, James D.  Head, Psychology Laboratories, Research
       Department, Central Institute for the Deaf, 818 South
       Euclid, St. Louis, Missouri 63110.

Nixon, Charles W.  Aerospace Medical Research Lab, 6570
       AMRL (BBA), Wright Patterson AFB, Ohio 45433.

Norris, Kenneth S.  Director, The Oceanic Institute, Makapuu
       Oceanic Center, Waimanalo, Hawaii 96795.

Potash, Lawrence.  Psychology Department, University of
       Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta Canada.

Segal, Migdon.  Analyst in Environmental Policy, Environmental
       Policy Division, Legislative Reference Service,
       Library of Congress.

Shaw, Elmer.  Analyst in Environmental Policy, Environmental
       Policy Division, Legislative Reference Service, Library
       of Congress, Washington, D. C.

Taylor, John P.  National Academy of Sciences, National
       Research Council, 2101 Constitution Ave., N.W.
       Washington, D. C. 20418.

Thompson, R. D.  U. S. Bureau of Sport Fisheries and Wildlife,
       Denver, Colorado.

Tombaugh, Larry.  National Science Foundation,  Washington, D. C.

Welch, Bruce L.  Friends of Psychiatric Research,  Incorporated,
       52 Wade Ave., Baltimore,  Maryland 21228.
               Organizations Providing

        Materials,  Information, and Assistance


Aircraft Noise Abatement.  Federal Aviation Administration,
       U. S. Department of Transportation, Washington, D. C.

Agricultural Research Center.  U. S. Department  of Agriculture,
       Beltsville,  Maryland.

Bell Aerospace Company.  Buffalo, New York.

-------
                                                         64


Bell Laboratories.  600 Mountain Ave., Murray Hill, New Jersey
       07974.

Blackwater National Wildlife Refuge.  Bureau of Sport
       Fisheries and Wildlife, Fish and Wildlife Service,
       U. S. Department of the Interior, Rt. 1, Box 121,
       Cambridge, Maryland 21613. .

Bureau of Sport Fisheries and Wildlife.  U. S. Department
       of the Interior, Washington,  D. ..C.

Citizens League Against the Sonic Boom, 19 Appleton Street,
       Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138.

Citizens for a Quieter City, Inc.  The American Red Cross
       Building, 150 Amsterdam Ave., New York, New York 10023.

Defenders of Wildlife.  2000 N Street, N. W., Washington,
       D. C. 20036.

Environmental Planning Division.  Housing and Urban Development,
       Washington, D. C.

Environmental Policy Division.  Legislative Reference Service,
       Library of Congress, Washington, D. C.

Federal Aviation Administration.  U. S. Department of
       Transportation, 800 Independence Avenue, Washington,
       D. C. 20590.

Langley Research Center.  U. S. National Aeronautics and Space
       Administration, Hampton, Virginia.

National Academy of Engineering.  Washington, D. C.

National Academy of Sciences.  National Research Council,
       Washington, D. C.

National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration.  Environmental
       Data Service, U. S. Department of Commerce, Rockville,
       Maryland 20852.

National Science Foundation.  Washington, D. C.

National Wildlife Federation.  1412 Sixteenth Street, N. W.,
       Washington, D. C. 20036.

Office of Environmental Quality.  Federal Aviation Administration,
       U. S. Department of Transportation, Washington, D. C.

Office of Noise Abatement.  Research Division, U. S. Department
       of Transportation, Washington, D. C.

-------
                                                         65
Patuxent Wildlife Research Center.  Bureau of Sport Fisheries
       and Wildlife, Fish and Wildlife Service, U. S.
       Department of the Interior, Laurel, Maryland 20810.

Urban Transportation Center.  Consortium of Universities,
       Washington, D. C.

Wildlife Management Institute.  Wire Building, Washington, D. C.
       20005.

The Wildlife Society.  3900 Wisconsin Ave., N. W., Suite S-176,
       Washington, D. C. 20016.

Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution.  Woods Hole, Massachusetts
       02543.

-------
                                                         66


                      References
Anthony, A. and Acker man, E.  Effects of noise on the blood
       eosinophil levels and adrenals of mice.  Journal of
       the Acoustical Society of America, 1955, 27, 1144-
         __
Anthony, A. and Acker man, E.  Biological effects of noise
       in vertebrate animals.  Wright Air Development Center,
       WADA Technical Report 57-647, November, 1957, 118 pp.

Anthony, A., Acker man, E. and Lloyd, J. A.  Noise stress in
       laboratory rodents.  I. Behavioral and endocrine
       response of mice, rats, and guinea pigs.  Journal
       of the Acoustical Society of America, 1959, 31,
Anthony, A. and Harclerode, J. E.  Noise stress in laboratory
       rodents.  II. Effects of chronic noise exposures on
       sexual performance and reproductive function of
       guinea pigs.  Journal of the Acoustical Society of
       America, 1959, 31, 1437^13415.

Aplin, J. A.  The effect of explosives on marine life.
       California Fish and Game, 1947, 33(1), 23-30.

Arkhepov, N. S., et al.  Biological effect of high-frequency
       ultrasound.  U. S. Department of Commerce, Joint
       Publication and Research Service, JPRS 47378,
       1969, 10 p.

Beagley, H. A.  Acoustic trauma in the guinea pig.  I.
       E lee trophysio logy and history.  Acta Oto-Lar yngologica ,
       1965a, 60, 437-451.

Beagley, H. A.  Acoustic trauma in the guinea pig.  II.
       Electron microscopy including the morphology of all
       junctions in the organ of Corti.  Acta Oto-Lary ngologica ,
       1965b, 60, 479-495.

Bell, W. B.  Animal response to sonic boom.  Paper presented
       at the 80th meeting of the Acoustical Society of
       America, Houston, November, 1970.

Benitez, L. D. , Eldredge, D. H. , and Templer, J. W.
       Electr ©physiological correlates of behavioral
       temporary threshold shifts in chinchilla.  Paper
       presented at the 80th meeting of the Acoustical
       Society of America, Houston, November, 1970.

-------
                                                         67
Block, B. C.  Williarasport Pennsylvania tries starling
       control with distress calls.  Pest Control,  1966,
       34, 24-30.

Bond, J.  Responses of man and lower animals to acoustical
       stimuli.  U. S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural
       Research Service, Animal and Poultry Husbandry
       Research Branch, Beltsville, Maryland, October 1, 1956.

Bond, J.  Effects of noise on the physiology and behavior of
       farm animals.  Physiological Effects of Noise,
       B. L. Welch and A. S. Welch (Eds.), 1570, New York:
       Plenum Press, 295-306.

Bond, J., Winchester, C. F., Campbell, L. E. and J. C. Webb.
       Effects of loud sounds on the physiology and behavior
       of swine.  U. S. Department of Agriculture,
       Agricultural Research Service Technical Bulletin,
       No. 1280.

Borisova, M. K.  The effect of noise on the conditioned reflex
       activity of animals.  Zhurnal Vysshei Deiatel* nosti im
       1. P. Pavlova, 1960, 10, 971-97eT

Brzezinska, Z.  Changes in acetylcholine concentration  in
       cerebral tissue in rats repeatedly exposed to  the
       action of mechanical vibration.  Acta Physiologica
       Polonica, 1968, 19, 810-815.

Casady, R. B. and Lehmann, R. P.  Responses of farm animals
       to sonic booms.  Sonic boom experiments at Edwards
       Air Force Base.  National Sonic Boom Evaluation  Office
       Interim Report, NS BE-1-67 of 28 July  1967.   Annex H.

Casady, R. B. and Lehmann, R. P.  Responses of farm animals
       to sonic booms, Sonic boom experiments at Edwards
       Air Force Base.   Annex H. U. S. Department of Agriculture,
       Agricultural Research Service, Animal Husbandry  Research
       Division, Beltsville, Maryland, 20 September 1966.

Committee on the Problem of Noise.  Final Report.  Presented
       to Parliament  July 1963.  London:  Her Majesty's
       Stationery Office, Cmnd. 2056, 19s bd. net.

Conti, A. and Borgo,  M.  Behaviour of cytochrome oxidase
       activity in the cochlea of the guinea pig following
       acoustic stimulation.  Acta Oto-Laryngologica,
       1964, 58, 321-330.

Coveil,  W. P.  Histologic changes in  the  organ of Corti with
       intense sound.  Journal of Comparative Neurology,
       1953, 99, 43-59.

-------
                                                         68
Crummett, J. G.  Acoustic information denial as a means for
       vertebrate pest control.  Paper presented at the 80th
       meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Houston,
       November, 1970.

Cummings, W. C.  Gray whales avoid sounds of killer whales.
       Fishery Bulletin, July, 1971  (in press).

Cutkomp, L. jv.  Effects of ultrasonic energy on storage
       insects.  Agriculture Department Cooperative State
       Research Service, Minnesota,  1969.

Davis, P.  Raven's response to sonic bang.  British Birds,
       1967, 60, 370.

Diehl, Fred P.  Sound as a rodent deterrent.  Pest Control,
       1969, 37, 36-44.

Dooling, R. J., Mulligan, J. A. and  J. D. Miller.  Relation
       of auditory sensitivity and song spectrum for the
       common canary.  Journal of. the Acoustical Society of
       America, (in press).

Elbowicz-Waniewska, Z.  Investigations on the influence of
       acoustic and ultraacoustic field on biochemical
       process.  Acta Physiologica Polonica, 1962, 13,
       362-370.

F A 0 Fisheries Report No. 76.  Report on a meeting for
       consultations on underwater noise.  Food and
       Agriculture Organization of the United Nations,
       April, 1970.

Fish, J. F. and Vania, J. S.  Killer whale, Orcinus orca,
       sounds repel white whales, Delphinapterus leucas.
       Fishery Bulletin, July, I97~T.

Fitch, J. E. and Young, P. H.  Use and effect of explosions
       in California coastal waters.  California Fish and
       Game, 1948, 34, 53-70.

Fitzwater, W. D.  Sonic systems for  bird control.  Pest Control,
       1970, 38, 9-16.

Friedman, M., Byers, S. 0. and A. E. Brown.  Plasma lipid
       responses of rats and rabbits to an auditory stimulus.
       American Journal of Physiology, 212, 1174-1178, 1967.

Frings, H. and Frings, M.  Recorded  calls of the eastern crow
       as attractants and repellents.  Journal of Wildlife
       Management. 1957, 21, 91.

-------
                                                         69


Frings, H. and Frings, M.  Reactions of swarms of Pentaneura
       aspera  (Diptera: Tendipedidae)  to sound.  Annals of
       the Entomological Society of America.  1959,  52,
       7Z5-733":

Frings, H. and Jumber, J.  Preliminary studies on the use of
       a specific sound to repel starlings  (Sturnus vulgaris)
       from objectionable roosts.  Science.  1954,119, 318-319.

Frings, H. and Little, F.  Reactions of honey bees  in the hive
       to simple sounds.  Science. 1957, 125, 122.

Geber, W. F., Anderson, T. A. and Van  Dyne,  B.  Physiologic
       response of the albino rat to chronic noise  stress.
       Archives £f Environmental Health, 1966, 12,  751-754.

Gonzalez, G., Miller, N. and Istre, C., Jr.   Influence of
       rocket noise upon hearing in guinea  pigs.  Aerospace
       Medicine. 1970, 41, 21-25.                 	

Graham, F.  Ear pollution.  Audubon, 1969,  71, 34-39.

Greaves, J. H. and Rowe, F. P.  Responses of confined rodent
       populations to an ultrasound generator.  Journal of
       Wildlife Management. 1969, 33,  409-417.  	

Griffin, D. R., McCue, J. J. G. and Grinnell, A. D.  The
       resistance of bats to jamming.  Journal of Experimental
       Zoology. 1963, 152, 229-250.    	

Groh, L. S.  The effects of two litter sizes and two levels
       of noise during infancy upon the adult behavior of
       the white rat.  Dissertation Abstracts. 1965, 27,
       598-599.	'   '

Henkin, H.  The death of birds.  Environment. 1969, 11, SI

Hill, E. P.  Bat control with high frequency sound.  Pest
       Control. 1970, 38, 18.                        	

Hiroshige, T., Sato, T., Ohta, R. and  Itoh,  S.  Increase of
       corticotropin-releasing activity in  the rat  hypothalamus
       following noxious stimuli.  The Japanese Journal of
       Physiology. 1969, 19, 866-875:

Hrubes, V.  Changes in concentration of non-esterified fatty
       acids in the rat plasma after load.   Activitas Nervosa
       Superior. 1964, 6, 60-62.            	

Hrubes, V. and Benes, V.  The influence of  repeated noise
       stress on rats.  Acta Biologica et Medica Germanica,
       1965, 15, 592-596":    ~"	—	

-------
                                                          70


 Ishii,  O. ,  Takahashi,  T.  and Balogh,  K.   Glycogen in the
        inner ear after acoustic stimulation.   Acta Oto-
        Laryngologica.  1969,  67,  573-582.

 Ishii,  H. and Yokobori, K.   Experimental  studies  on teratogenic
        activity  of  noise  stimulation.  Gunma  Journal of
        Medical Sciences,  1960,  9,  153-ltjT

 Jeannoutot,  D. W. and  Adams,  J.  L.  Progesterone  versus
        treatment by high  intensity sound  as methods of
        controlling  broodiness in broad breasted bronze
        turkeys.   Poultry  Science.  1961, 40, 512-521.

 Jensen, M.  M.  and Rasmussen, A.  F.  Audiogenic stress and
        susceptibility  to  infection.  Physiological Effects
        of Noise,  B. L. Welch and A. S. Welch  (Eds.),  1970,
Jurtshuk, P., We It man, A. S. and Sackler, A. M.  Biochemical
       response of rats to auditory stress.  Science,  1959,
       129, 1424-1425.

Kirkpatrick, R. L. and Harein, P. K.   Inhibition of
       reproduction of Indian-Meal Moths, Plodia inter punctel la.
       by exposure to amplified sound.  Journal~of Economic
       Entomology, 1965, 58, 920-921.

Klein, D. R.  Reaction of reindeer to  obstructions and
       disturbances.  Science, 1971, 173, 393-398.

Konishi, Masakazu.  Comparative neurophysiological studies
       of hearing and vocalizations in songbirds.  Zeitschrift
       fuer Vergleichende Physiologie, 1970, 67, 363-381.

Langowski, D. J. , Wight, H. M. and Jacobson, J. N.  Responses
       of instrumentally conditioned starlings to aversive
       acoustic stimuli.  Journal of Wildlife Management,
       1969, 33, 669-677.

Lawrence, M. and Yantis, P.  Individual differences in
       functional recovery and structural repair following
       over stimulation of the guinea pig.  Annals of Otology,
       Rhino logy, and Laryngology, 1957, 66, 595-6"2T.

Lindgren, D. L.  Maintaining marketability of stored grain
       and cereal products.  Agriculture Department
       Cooperative State Research Service, California, 1969.

Lindzey,  G.   Emotionality and audiogenic seizure susceptibility
       in five inbred strains of mice.  Journal of
       Comparative and Physiological Psycho logy ,"T!951, 44,
       389-393.

-------
                                                         71
Little, H. F.  Reactions of honey bees to oscillations of
       known frequency.  Anatomical Record, 1959, 134, 601.

Majeau-Chargois, D. A., Berlin, C. I. and G. D. Whitehouse.
       Sonic boom effects on the organ of Corti.  The
       Laryngoscope, 1970, 80, 620-630.

Malar, T. and Kleerekoper, H.  Observations on some effects
       of sound intensity on locomotor patterns of naive
       goldfish.  American Zoologist. 1968, 8, 741-742.

Manley, Geoffrey.  Comparative studies of auditory physiology
       in reptiles.  Zeitschrift fuer Vergleichende Physiologic.
       1970, 67, 363-3irn

Messersmith, D. H.  Control of bird depredation.  Agriculture
       Department Cooperative State Research Service,
       Maryland, 1970.

Miline, R., Devecerski, V. and R. Krstic.  Effects of auditory
       stimuli on the pineal gland of the bat during
       hibernation.  Acta Anatomica, 1969, 73, Suppl. 56,
       293-300.

Miller, J. D., Rothenberg, S. J. and Eldredge, D. H.
       Preliminary observations on the effects of exposure
       to noise for seven days on the hearing and inner
       ear of the chinchilla.  The Journal of the Acoustical
       Society of America (in press).

Miller, J. D., Watson, C. S. and Covell, W. P.  Deafening
       effects of noise on the cat.  Acta Oto-laryngologica,
       Suppl. 176, 1963, 91 pp.

Monaenkov, A. M.  Influence of prolonged stimulation  by
       sound of an electric bell on conditioned-reflex
       activity in mammals.  Zhur. Vyssh. Nervn. DeitaP
       6, 891-897, 1956.  PsycEoIogical Abstracts, 52. 1958.

Monastyrskaya, B. I., Prakh'e, I. B., and Khaunina, R. A.
       Effect of acoustic stimulation on the pituitary
       adrenal system in healthy rats and rats genetically
       sensitive to sound.  Bulletin of Experimental  Biology
       and Medicine (transl. fromRuss.), 1969, 68, 1357-1360.

Ogle, C. W. and Lockett, M. F.  The release of neurohypo-
       physical hormone by sound.  Journal of Endocrinology,
       1966, 36, 281-290.          	'	'—	**•

Osintseva, V. P., Pushkina, N. N;, Bonashevskaya, T.  I., and
       Kaverina, V. F.  Noise induced changes in the  adrenals.
       Hygiene and Sanitation. 1969, 34, 147-151.

-------
                                                          72
Parker, J. B. and Bayley, N. D.   Investigations on effects
       of aircraft sound on milk  production of dairy
       cattle, 1957-1958.  United States Department of
       Agriculture, Agriculture Research Service, Animal
       Husbandry Research Division,  1960, 22 pp.

Pearson, E. W., Skon, P. R. and Corner, G. W.  Dispersal of
       urban roosts with records  of  starling distress calls.
       Journal of Wildlife Management, 1967, 31, 502-506.

Peters, E. N.  Temporary shifts in auditory thresholds of
       chinchilla after exposure  to  noise.  The Journal of
       the Accoustical Society of America, 19^5, 37, 831^533.

Poche, L. B., Stockwell, C. W. and Ades, H.  Cochlear hair
       cell damage in guinea pigs after exposure to impulse
       noise.  The Journal of the Acoustical Society of
       America,'~r9'69, 46, 9Tf-"9^T.

Ponomar'kov, V. I., Tysik, Yu, Kidryavtseva, V. I., Barer,
       A. S., Kostin, V. K., Leshchenko, V. Ye., Morozova,
       R. M., Nosokin, L. V., Frolov, A. N.  NASA TT F-529i,
       "Problems of Space Biology,"  Vol. 7, Operational
       Activity, Problems of Habitability and Biotechnology,
       NASA, May 1969.

Potash, L. M.  A signal detection problem and possible
       solution in Japanese quail.   Animal Behavior (in press)

Pye, J. D.  Ultrasonic bioacoustics, Final Scientific Report,
       llth May, 1965 - 30th June, 1970.  U. S. Government
       Research and Reports Index 1971, No. 1, p. SU-2, No.
       AD 714 632

Sewell, G. D.  Ultrasonic signals from rodents.  Ultrasonics,
       1970, 8, 26-30.                           	

Shaw, E. W.  California Condor.   Library of Congress
       Legislative Reference Service, 1970, SK351, 70-127.

Shulov, A. S.  Acoustic responses of locusts—Schistocera,
       Dociostarus, and Aerotylus.   U. S. Dept of Agriculture,
       Agricultural Research Service, Entomology Research
       Division, 1969.

Singh, K.  B. and Rao, P.  Studies on the polycystic ovaries
       of rats under continuous auditory stress.  American
       Journal Obstetrics and Gynecology, 1970, 108, 557-564.

Sprock, C. M., Howard, W. E., and  Jacob, F. C.  Sound as a
       deterrent to rats and mice.   Journal of Wildlife
       Management, 1967, 31, 729-741"!

-------
                                                         73
Stadelman, W. J.  The effect of sounds of varying intensity
       on hatchability of chicken egg.  Poultry Science.
       1958a, 37, 166-169.              	

Stadelman, W. J.  Observations with growing chickens on
       the effects of sounds of varying intensities.
       Poultry Science, 1958b, 37, 776-779.

Thompson, R. D., Grant, C. V., Pearson, E. W., and Corner, G. W.
       Cardiac response of starlings to sound: effects of
       lighting and grouping.  American Journal of Physiology,
       1968a, 214, 41-44.	*"

Thompson, R. D., Grant, C. V., Pearson, E. W., and Corner, G. W.
       Differential heart rate response of starlings to
       sound stimuli of biological origin.  The Journal of
       Wildlife Management, 1968b, 32, 888-8U3T

Thompson, W. D. and Sontag, L. W.  Behavioral effects in the
       offspring of rats subjected to audiogenic seizures
       during the gestational period.  Journal of Comparative
       and Physiological Psychology. 1956, 49, "J54-456.

Thorpe, W. H.  The significance of vocal imitation in
       animals with special reference to birds.  Acta
       Biologica Experimentia. 1969, 29, 251-269.

Travis, H. F., Richardson, G. V., Menear, J. R. and Bond, J.
       The effects of simulated sonic booms on reproduction
       and behavior of farm-raised mink.  ARS 44-200, June
       1968, U. S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural
       Research Service.

Treptow, K.  Dynamics of glycemic reactions after repeated
       exposure to noise.  Activitas Nervosa Superior. 1966,
       8, 215-216.	*	

Tsao, C.  Perception and behavioral effects of sound in the
       Indian-Meal Moth.  U. S. Dept. of Agriculture,
       Agriculture Research Service, Market Quality Research
       Division, 1969.

United States Department of the Interior.  Environmental
       impact of the Big Cypress Swamp Jetport.  September,
       1969, 155 pp.

Vince, M. A.  Artificial acceleration of hatching in quail
       embryos.  Animal Behavior. 1966, 14, 389-394.

Ward, C. 0., Barletta, M. A., Kaye, T.  Teratogenic effects
       of audiogenic stress in albino mice.  Journal of
       Pharmaceutical Sciencest 1970, 59, 1661-1662.

-------
                                                         74
Ward, W. D.  and Nelson, D. A.  Reduction of permanent
       threshold shifts through intermittency.  Paper
       presented at the 80th meeting of the Acoustical
       Society of America, Houston, November, 1970.

Werner, R.  Influence of sound on the intermediary lobe
       of the rat hypophysis.  Compte Rendus cte 1*Association
       des Anatomistes, 1959, 45, 78-7557

Wight, H. M.  Development and testing of methods for
       repelling starlings that roost in holly.  Oregon
       State Government, 1971.

Yeakel, E. H., Shenkin, H. A., Rothballer, A. B., McCann, S. M.
       Adrenalectomy and blood pressure of rats subjected
       to auditory stimulation.  Journal of Physiology, 1948,
       155, 118-127.

Zondek, B.  Effect of auditory stimuli on female reproductive
       organs.  New England Obstetrical and Gynecological
       Society, 1964, 18, 177-185.

Zondek, B. and Isachar, T.  Effect of audiogenic stimulation
       on genital function and reproduction.  Acta
       Endocrinologica, 1964, 45, 227-234.

Zoric, V.  Effects of sound on mouse testes.  Acta Anatomica,
       1959, 38, 176.

-------